Agustín Espinosa
Pontifical Catholic University of Peru
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Agustín Espinosa.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2016
Vivian L. Vignoles; Ellinor Owe; Maja Becker; Peter B. Smith; Matthew J. Easterbrook; Rupert Brown; Roberto González; Nicolas Didier; Diego Carrasco; Maria Paz Cadena; Siugmin Lay; Seth J. Schwartz; Sabrina E. Des Rosiers; Juan A. Villamar; Alin Gavreliuc; Martina Zinkeng; Robert Kreuzbauer; Peter Baguma; Mariana Martin; Alexander Tatarko; Ginette Herman; Isabelle de Sauvage; Marie Courtois; Ragna B. Garðarsdóttir; Charles Harb; Inge Schweiger Gallo; Paula Prieto Gil; Raquel Lorente Clemares; Gabriella Campara; George Nizharadze
Markus and Kitayamas (1991) theory of independent and interdependent self-construals had a major influence on social, personality, and developmental psychology by highlighting the role of culture in psychological processes. However, research has relied excessively on contrasts between North American and East Asian samples, and commonly used self-report measures of independence and interdependence frequently fail to show predicted cultural differences. We revisited the conceptualization and measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals in 2 large-scale multinational surveys, using improved methods for cross-cultural research. We developed (Study 1: N = 2924 students in 16 nations) and validated across cultures (Study 2: N = 7279 adults from 55 cultural groups in 33 nations) a new 7-dimensional model of self-reported ways of being independent or interdependent. Patterns of global variation support some of Markus and Kitayamas predictions, but a simple contrast between independence and interdependence does not adequately capture the diverse models of selfhood that prevail in different world regions. Cultural groups emphasize different ways of being both independent and interdependent, depending on individualism-collectivism, national socioeconomic development, and religious heritage. Our 7-dimensional model will allow future researchers to test more accurately the implications of cultural models of selfhood for psychological processes in diverse ecocultural contexts. (PsycINFO Database Record
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2013
Ellinor Owe; Vivian L. Vignoles; Maja Becker; Rupert Brown; Peter B. Smith; Spike W. S. Lee; Matthew J. Easterbrook; Tanuja Gadre; Xiao Zhang; Mirona Gheorghiu; Peter Baguma; Alexander Tatarko; Said Aldhafri; Martina Zinkeng; Seth J. Schwartz; Sabrina E. Des Rosiers; Juan A. Villamar; Kassahun Habtamu Mekonnen; Camillo Regalia; Claudia Manzi; Maria Brambilla; Ersin Kusdil; Selinay Çaǧlar; Alin Gavreliuc; Mariana Martin; Zhang Jian-xin; Shaobo Lv; Ronald Fischer; Taciano L. Milfont; Ana Raquel Rosas Torres
Beliefs about personhood are understood to be a defining feature of individualism-collectivism (I-C), but they have been insufficiently explored, given the emphasis of research on values and self-construals. We propose the construct of contextualism, referring to beliefs about the importance of context in understanding people, as a facet of cultural collectivism. A brief measure was developed and refined across 19 nations (Study 1: N = 5,241), showing good psychometric properties for cross-cultural use and correlating well at the nation level with other supposed facets and indicators of I-C. In Study 2 (N = 8,652), nation-level contextualism predicted ingroup favoritism, corruption, and differential trust of ingroup and outgroup members, while controlling for other facets of I-C, across 35 nations. We conclude that contextualism is an important part of cultural collectivism. This highlights the importance of beliefs alongside values and self-representations and contributes to a wider understanding of cultural processes.
Revista De Psicologia Social | 2010
Elza Techio; Magdalena Bobowik; Darío Páez; James H. Liu; Elena Zubieta; Agustín Espinosa
Abstract This study analyzes how people perceive world history on three continents: Latin America, Europe and Africa. A total of 1179 university students form Argentina, Brazil, Peru, Portugal, Spain, Guinea-Bissau, and Cape Verde were asked to evaluate world events and leaders in terms of their valence and importance. The results demonstrated that social representations of history show a Euro/North American-centric, long-term positive evaluation, recency, and socio-centric bias. Euro/North American-centric events and leaders were found to be rated as more important and were more positively perceived in general. Distant political events, like French or American Revolution, were considered to be more positive than XX century similar events, which supports the long-term positive evaluation bias hypothesis. The hypothesis on recency bias was partially substantiated. Confirming the existence of such bias, World War II was rated as more important than the previous XX century wars and revolutions. Socio-centric bias also received partial support. African participants rated Mandela as a more important leader than other participants did. Latin Americans rated Che Guevara less positively, which suggests that some leaders are generally idealized icons, not based on group belongingness. However, results did not bring support to the centrality of war hypothesis. Wars were indeed negatively evaluated and World War II was rated as an important and negative event. Nevertheless, war- and politics-related events were not perceived as more important than the Industrial Revolution, suggesting that people appraise the importance of long-term socioeconomic factors of history when responding to close-ended quantitative measures (vs. open-ended salience measures). Results are discussed in the framework of social representations of history.
International Journal of Psychology | 2016
Peter B. Smith; Vivian L. Vignoles; Maja Becker; Ellinor Owe; Matthew J. Easterbrook; Rupert Brown; David Bourguignon; Ragna B. Garðarsdóttir; Robert Kreuzbauer; Boris Cendales Ayala; Masaki Yuki; Jianxin Zhang; Shaobo Lv; Phatthanakit Chobthamkit; Jas Laile Suzana Binti Jaafar; Ronald Fischer; Taciano L. Milfont; Alin Gavreliuc; Peter Baguma; Michael Harris Bond; Mariana Martin; Nicolay Gausel; Seth J. Schwartz; Sabrina E. Des Rosiers; Alexander Tatarko; Roberto González; Nicolas Didier; Diego Carrasco; Siugmin Lay; George Nizharadze
Variations in acquiescence and extremity pose substantial threats to the validity of cross-cultural research that relies on survey methods. Individual and cultural correlates of response styles when using 2 contrasting types of response mode were investigated, drawing on data from 55 cultural groups across 33 nations. Using 7 dimensions of self-other relatedness that have often been confounded within the broader distinction between independence and interdependence, our analysis yields more specific understandings of both individual- and culture-level variations in response style. When using a Likert-scale response format, acquiescence is strongest among individuals seeing themselves as similar to others, and where cultural models of selfhood favour harmony, similarity with others and receptiveness to influence. However, when using Schwartzs (2007) portrait-comparison response procedure, acquiescence is strongest among individuals seeing themselves as self-reliant but also connected to others, and where cultural models of selfhood favour self-reliance and self-consistency. Extreme responding varies less between the two types of response modes, and is most prevalent among individuals seeing themselves as self-reliant, and in cultures favouring self-reliance. As both types of response mode elicit distinctive styles of response, it remains important to estimate and control for style effects to ensure valid comparisons.
Psicologia & Sociedade | 2012
Karen Genna; Agustín Espinosa
The study analyzes stereotypical representations of Peruvian Ethnic Groups in a sample of middle class adults from Lima (n=130). Additionally, relationship between Ethnic Identification and Social Welfare is analyzed. In respect to stereotypical contents, it is observed the ambivalent representation of high and low status groups which are differentiated by their access to power and resources. The high status group includes White-Peruvians perceived as successful and non-patriotic people. Participants show high identification to this group and this identification is directly related to Social Welfare. On the other hand, Andeans, Amazonians and Afro-Peruvians were perceived as less successful but more affective and expressive groups. Participants show lower identification with these groups and these identifications are no related to Social Welfare.
Self and Identity | 2018
Maja Becker; Vivian L. Vignoles; Ellinor Owe; Matthew J. Easterbrook; Rupert Brown; Peter B. Smith; Sami Abuhamdeh; Boris Cendales Ayala; Ragna B. Garðarsdóttir; Ana Raquel Rosas Torres; Leoncio Camino; Michael Harris Bond; George Nizharadze; Benjamin Amponsah; Inge Schweiger Gallo; Paula Prieto Gil; Raquel Lorente Clemares; Gabriella Campara; Agustín Espinosa; Masaki Yuki; Xiao Zhang; Jianxin Zhang; Martina Zinkeng; Juan A. Villamar; Ersin Kusdil; Selinay Çağlar; Camillo Regalia; Claudia Manzi; Maria Brambilla; David Bourguignon
Abstract Self-continuity – the sense that one’s past, present, and future are meaningfully connected – is considered a defining feature of personal identity. However, bases of self-continuity may depend on cultural beliefs about personhood. In multilevel analyses of data from 7287 adults from 55 cultural groups in 33 nations, we tested a new tripartite theoretical model of bases of self-continuity. As expected, perceptions of stability, sense of narrative, and associative links to one’s past each contributed to predicting the extent to which people derived a sense of self-continuity from different aspects of their identities. Ways of constructing self-continuity were moderated by cultural and individual differences in mutable (vs. immutable) personhood beliefs – the belief that human attributes are malleable. Individuals with lower mutability beliefs based self-continuity more on stability; members of cultures where mutability beliefs were higher based self-continuity more on narrative. Bases of self-continuity were also moderated by cultural variation in contextualized (vs. decontextualized) personhood beliefs, indicating a link to cultural individualism-collectivism. Our results illustrate the cultural flexibility of the motive for self-continuity.
Cadernos De Pesquisa | 2015
Maitane Arnoso Martinez; Darío Páez Rovira; Manuel Cárdenas; Elena Zubieta; Agustín Espinosa; Marian Bilbao
This article analyzed 5 surveys conducted in Latin American countries (N = 4900) regarding how people perceive and cope with collective violence during dictatorship periods. There is general agreement on the need to remember the past, where victims of collective violence and the left-wing showed a more positive attitude, reporting the necessity of sharing the facts and having a more positive evaluation of measures of transitional justice. Even when official apologies show a critical perception, a positive view of them is associated with a better perception of the emotional climate. While the sincerity and perceived effectiveness of apologies are higher among victims and the left-wing in Argentina and Paraguay, in Chile the opposite is true. Results are discussed in the framework of social representations of the past. JustiCe • Latin america • truth commissions • sociaLEste artigo explorou as opinioes de uma amostra da populacao de cinco paises latino-americanos (N= 4900) no tocante a forma de abordar o passado politico relacionado as violacoes dos direitos humanos. Em geral, constata-se um amplo acordo quanto a necessidade de recordar o passado, sendo esse acordo maior entre as vitimas e as pessoas de esquerda, que, alem disso, manifestam maior necessidade de compartilhar socialmente os fatos e sao as que mais valorizam as medidas de justica transicional implementadas. Embora as desculpas sejam avaliadas de forma critica em geral, uma visao mais positiva das mesmas foi associada a uma melhor percepcao do clima emocional. Enquanto na Argentina e no Paraguai a sinceridade e eficacia percebidas nas desculpas sao maiores entre as vitimas e as pessoas de esquerda, no caso chileno a relacao e inversa. Os resultados sao discutidos no contexto das representacoes sociais do passado.
Revista De Psicologia Social | 2017
Manuel Pacheco; Agustín Espinosa; Mathias Schmitz
Abstract The purpose of this study is to analyse the influence of different musical genres with Andean influences (Huayno, Chicha and Rock/Fusion) on the components of Peruvian national identity. To this end, we enlisted the participation of 92 upper-middle-class people (M = 19.91, SD = 1.95). The participants were randomly assigned each music genre in three experimental groups: Huayno (n = 30), Chicha (n = 31) and Rock/Fusion (n = 31). The results show that Rock/Fusion is the most highly rated musical genre with the most positive influence on some stereotypical components of national identity, attitude towards music and positive emotions. In contrast, the more ‘Andeanized’ genres, especially Chicha music, are perceived more negatively and have fewer positive effects on the stereotypical components of national identity, attitude towards music and positive emotions. The discussion suggests that the influence of the musical expressions evaluated depends on the ascribed status of the social group where these expressions were originally conceived and are currently consumed.
Revista De Psicologia Social | 2016
Manuel Cárdenas; Elena Zubieta; Darío Páez; Maitane Arnoso; Agustín Espinosa
Abstract The socio-demographic and psychosocial approval determinants of the work conducted by the truth and reconciliation commissions (TRC) are analysed based on original survey data from Chilean, Argentinian and Peruvian samples (N = 2,947). A linear multiple regression analysis was carried out (R2 between .28 and .44; f2 between .45 and .78) which reveals the positive effect of commission functions perceived achievement, mainly the knowledge of truth (β = .28), justice (β = .15) and the contribution in creating a common history (β = .15). An analysis by country reveals in Chile salient variables such as institutional trust, political ideas, institutional apologies and hope and sadness emotions. In Argentina, together with TRC functions, the degree of information about the work of the commission and the perception of a negative social climate appear as relevant predictors. As regards Peru, the degree of exposure to violence appears as a good predictor of TRC’s work support. The results show the relevance of TRC functions’ fulfillment, as well as the role of institutional variables in the approval of the work they have done.
Journal of Prevention & Intervention in The Community | 2015
Agustín Espinosa; Tesania Velázquez
During decades of the 1980s and ‘90s, Peru went through one of the worst social, political, and economic crises in its Republican history. This period, marked by corruption, institutional weakening...