Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Andrei Gorea is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Andrei Gorea.


Journal of The Optical Society of America A-optics Image Science and Vision | 1986

New look at Bloch's law for contrast

Andrei Gorea; Christopher W. Tyler

It has been commonly reported that the temporal integration of grating contrast proceeds more slowly as spatial frequency is increased. Such results have been based on the critical duration for sensitivity to contrast pulses varying in duration, but the analyses have not assumed full integration at short durations and have neglected the effects of probability summation over time. To take such effects into account, we discuss a class of analytical models based on nonlinear temporal integration. On the assumption that the temporal impulse response of the visual system determines contrast integration over time, we develop both a high-threshold model and a signal-detection approach involving multiple and independent nonlinear signal detectors with a time-limited integration span. The redefined critical durations predicted by the models and verified by the data are about 35 msec and vary by no more than 10 msec across spatial frequency. This variation is entirely attributable to a change in the strength of inhibition with spatial frequency, and the analysis implies that the excitatory component is constant at all spatial frequencies, contrary to previous accounts.


Journal of The Optical Society of America A-optics Image Science and Vision | 1989

Motion processing by chromatic and achromatic visual pathways.

Andrei Gorea; Thomas V. Papathomas

We describe a family of stimuli consisting of colored bars of different orientations, which, when presented in rapid succession, may elicit unambiguous motion perception. These stimuli permitted the isolation of directional spatiotemporal information extracted from oriented luminance clues, from nonoriented chromatic-plus-luminance clues, or, when the stimuli were presented under equiluminant conditions, from pure chromatic clues. As a general rule, matching of orientation induces weaker motion-detection performances than does matching of color. When the orientation clues are in competition with the chromatic ones, motion perception based on the former is always overridden by motion perception based on the latter. We indirectly isolated an oriented chromatic mechanism that also contributes to motion perception. We finally showed that, under equiluminant conditions, matching of orientation across different colors is inefficient in eliciting motion perception, either because motion information is extracted poorly across different chromatic channels or because such channels show little orientational selectivity. Because motion strength determined by each of the manipulated attributes follows different functions with the displacement (or velocity) of the stimuli, we propose the existence of three underlying mechanisms, a luminance mechanism, a chromatic-plus-luminance mechanism, and a pure chromatic mechanism, each of which provides motion information.


Vision Research | 1991

Two carriers for motion perception: Color and luminance

Thomas V. Papathomas; Andrei Gorea; Bela Julesz

Starting with the experiments of Ramachandran and Gregory (Nature, 275, 55-56, 1978), several psychophysical studies in apparent motion (AM) have established that the perception of motion is significantly impaired at equiluminance. Still debated, however, is whether color alone can resolve ambiguities in AM. We report here on several psychophysical experiments, the quantitative results of which indicate that color does play a substantial role in AM. These findings seem to support recently proposed neurophysiological frameworks according to which there exist significant interactions among the neuronal pathways mediating the perception of basic visual attributes such as color, motion, form and depth.


Nature Neuroscience | 2001

Disentangling signal from noise in visual contrast discrimination

Andrei Gorea; Dov Sagi

Human ability to detect stimulus changes (ΔC) decreases with increasing reference level (C). Because detection performance reflects the signal-to-noise ratio within the relevant sensory brain module, this behavior can be accounted for in two extreme ways: first, the internal response change ΔR evoked by a constant ΔC decreases with C (that is, the transducer R = f(C) displays a compressive nonlinearity), whereas the internal noise is independent of R; second, ΔR is constant with C but the noise level increases with R. A newly discovered constraint on human decision-making helps solve this century-old problem: in a detection task where multiple changes occur with equal probabilities, observers use a unique response criterion to decide whether a change has occurred. For contrast discrimination, our results supported the first account above: human performance was limited by the contrast transducer nonlinearity and an almost constant noise.


Psychological Science | 2013

Time in Perspective

Andrei Gorea; Janice Hau

Perceptions of time and space are subject to strong contextual effects. Like their physical counterparts, they appear to be bound together. The perceived spatial extent of a constant retinal extent increases with its perceived distance from the observer. The perceived duration of a moving object increases with its covered angular trajectory. It follows that the perceived duration of moving objects covering identical angular trajectories should also increase with distance. Using three-dimensionally rendered balls rolling for 600 ms, 900 ms, and 1,200 ms and covering 5.5°, 11°, and 22° trajectories in fronto-parallel planes of a linear-perspective scene, we showed that perceived duration dilates by up to 50% as the fronto-parallel plane of the rolling ball recedes from the observer. Such time dilation is mostly contributed to by the smaller size of the distant ball. As in a three-dimensional world, objects’ sizes and their covered trajectories per time unit decrease with distance, and as the two factors lead to opposite perceived-duration effects, the results suggest a form of time constancy in a three-dimensional world.


Vision Research | 1978

Complete adaptation to patterned stimuli: a necessary and sufficient condition for Weber's law for contrast.

Jj Kulikowski; Andrei Gorea

Abstract Incremental (masking) and adaptation contrast thresholds were measured for sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequencies presented either in an on-off mode or reversed in contrast, at different temporal rates. It is shown that whenever the masking grating stimulates other mechanisms than those concerned with the detection of the test stimulus, the ratio of Δ contrast (test)/contrast (mask) is not constant, thereby indicating a departure from Webers law. It is concluded that Webers law directly reflects complete adaptation of the stimulated pattern and/or movement mechanism.


Vision Research | 1998

Motion blur and motion sharpening: temporal smear and local contrast non-linearity

Stephen T. Hammett; Mark A. Georgeson; Andrei Gorea

Blurred images may appear sharper when drifting than when stationary. But, paradoxically, moving sharp edges may appear more blurred. To resolve this paradox, the perceived sharpness of drifting, blurred, square wave gratings was compared with that of their static analogues over a range of speeds, blurs and spatial frequencies. Both motion blur and motion sharpening occurred, depending upon the physical blur of the patterns. For large extents of blur (> 10 min arc) moving patterns always appeared sharper than their static analogues, but for small blurs (< 10 min arc) moving edges appeared more blurred than stationary ones. We present a quantitative model for the distortion of waveforms in motion based on two factors: (i) visual temporal integration that smears moving images, and (ii) a local contrast non-linearity that increasingly sharpens the effective profile of edges as speed and contrast increase. We suggest that a plausible account of the speed-dependent non-linearity is the differential recruitment of M and P cells at different speeds.


systems man and cybernetics | 1997

A human vision based computational model for chromatic texture segregation

Thomas V. Papathomas; Ramanujan S. Kashi; Andrei Gorea

We have developed a computational model for texture perception which has physiological relevance and correlates well with human performance. The model attempts to simulate the visual processing characteristics by incorporating mechanisms tuned to detect luminance-polarity, orientation, spatial frequency and color, which are characteristic features of any textural image. We obtained a very good correlation between the models simulation results and data from psychophysical experiments with a systematically selected set of visual stimuli with texture patterns defined by spatial variations in color, luminance, and orientation. In addition, the model predicts correctly texture segregation performance with key benchmarks and natural textures. This represents a first effort to incorporate chromatic signals in texture segregation models of psychophysical relevance, most of which have treated grey-level images so far. Another novel feature of the model is the extension or the concept of spatial double opponency to domains beyond color, such as orientation and spatial frequency. The model has potential applications in the areas of image processing, machine vision and pattern recognition, and scientific visualization.


Perception | 1990

Context Superiority in a Detection Task with Line-Element Stimuli: A Low-Level Effect

Andrei Gorea; Bela Julesz

Detection and identification performances for vertical and horizontal target elements embedded within an array of oriented noise elements were measured as a function of the orientation difference between the target and noise elements. Detection performances obtained with one vertical and three horizontal target elements clustered together and displayed such that they formed a schematic face-like pattern were significantly better than those obtained with the same clustered target elements displayed in an arbitrary, symmetrical or asymmetrical, configuration. This was so even though the identification of the face and nonface stimuli was well below the detection threshold of their parts. Detection thresholds for the clustered nonface patterns were slightly but significantly lower than those for the same target elements dispersed among the noise elements. Probability summation calculations based on the detection results obtained with one single target element predict detection thresholds which are intermediate between those of the clustered and dispersed targets, suggesting that inhibitory and facilitatory spatial interactions respectively are at work for the two types of stimuli. The existence of a context(face)-superiority effect at the detection level indicates top-down/bottom-up interactions between remote visual processing stages.


Journal of Vision | 2009

Adaptation and prolonged inhibition as a main cause of motion-induced blindness

Andrei Gorea; Florent Caetta

Motion-induced blindness (MIB) is one of the most enigmatic perceptual disappearance phenomena. Here we suggest that MIB may be caused by the combined effects of two distinct adaptation processes: one shared with two other non-MIB configurations and entailing a response-gain reduction, and a second, MIB-specific transient-to-sustained incremental inhibition causing a contrast-gain reduction. Response-gain reduction is evidenced by brightness-tracking experiment where the 1-minute brightness time course of an MIB target is compared to the time courses of the same target superimposed on a static mask (SM) and on no mask at all (absent mask; AM). MIB and SM yield about the same brightness time courses with a faster initial drop and reaching a lower plateau than AM. While the frequency of phenomenal suppressions and their duration are very much reduced under SM and AM conditions, they increase as for MIB within the first 5-15 s of inspection and level off thereafter. Contrast-gain reduction over time is evidenced in a detection experiment showing that MIB target thresholds are higher and increase more steeply with inspection time than AM or SM thresholds. The interplay between these noisy adaptation and prolonged inhibition processes may well account for MIBs specificity.

Collaboration


Dive into the Andrei Gorea's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Thomas V. Papathomas

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dov Sagi

Weizmann Institute of Science

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Pedro Cardoso-Leite

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Florian Waszak

Paris Descartes University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Pascal Mamassian

Paris Descartes University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Delphine Rider

Paris Descartes University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Florent Caetta

Paris Descartes University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jean Lorenceau

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge