Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Andrés Canales-Johnson is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Andrés Canales-Johnson.


PLOS Computational Biology | 2014

Spectral Signatures of Reorganised Brain Networks in Disorders of Consciousness

Srivas Chennu; Paola Finoia; Evelyn Kamau; Judith Allanson; Guy B. Williams; Martin M. Monti; Valdas Noreika; Aurina Arnatkeviciute; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Francisco Olivares; Daniela Cabezas-Soto; David K. Menon; John D. Pickard; Adrian M. Owen; Tristan A. Bekinschtein

Theoretical advances in the science of consciousness have proposed that it is concomitant with balanced cortical integration and differentiation, enabled by efficient networks of information transfer across multiple scales. Here, we apply graph theory to compare key signatures of such networks in high-density electroencephalographic data from 32 patients with chronic disorders of consciousness, against normative data from healthy controls. Based on connectivity within canonical frequency bands, we found that patient networks had reduced local and global efficiency, and fewer hubs in the alpha band. We devised a novel topographical metric, termed modular span, which showed that the alpha network modules in patients were also spatially circumscribed, lacking the structured long-distance interactions commonly observed in the healthy controls. Importantly however, these differences between graph-theoretic metrics were partially reversed in delta and theta band networks, which were also significantly more similar to each other in patients than controls. Going further, we found that metrics of alpha network efficiency also correlated with the degree of behavioural awareness. Intriguingly, some patients in behaviourally unresponsive vegetative states who demonstrated evidence of covert awareness with functional neuroimaging stood out from this trend: they had alpha networks that were remarkably well preserved and similar to those observed in the controls. Taken together, our findings inform current understanding of disorders of consciousness by highlighting the distinctive brain networks that characterise them. In the significant minority of vegetative patients who follow commands in neuroimaging tests, they point to putative network mechanisms that could support cognitive function and consciousness despite profound behavioural impairment.


PLOS ONE | 2014

How do you feel when you can't feel your body? Interoception, functional connectivity and emotional processing in depersonalization-derealization disorder.

Lucas Sedeño; Blas Couto; Margherita Melloni; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Adrián Yoris; Sandra Baez; Sol Esteves; Marcela Velásquez; Pablo Barttfeld; Mariano Sigman; Rafael Kichic; Dante R. Chialvo; Facundo Manes; Tristan A. Bekinschtein; Agustín Ibáñez

Depersonalization-Derealization Disorder (DD) typically manifests as a disruption of body self-awareness. Interoception −defined as the cognitive processing of body signals− has been extensively considered as a key processing for body self-awareness. In consequence, the purpose of this study was to investigate whether there are systematic differences in interoception between a patient with DD and controls that might explain the disembodiment symptoms suffered in this disease. To assess interoception, we utilized a heartbeat detection task and measures of functional connectivity derived from fMRI networks in interoceptive/exteroceptivo/mind-wandering states. Additionally, we evaluated empathic abilities to test the association between interoception and emotional experience. The results showed patients impaired performance in the heartbeat detection task when compared to controls. Furthermore, regarding functional connectivity, we found a lower global brain connectivity of the patient relative to controls only in the interoceptive state. He also presented a particular pattern of impairments in affective empathy. To our knowledge, this is the first experimental research that assesses the relationship between interoception and DD combining behavioral and neurobiological measures. Our results suggest that altered neural mechanisms and cognitive processes regarding body signaling might be engaged in DD phenomenology. Moreover, our study contributes experimental data to the comprehension of brain-body interactions and the emergence of self-awareness and emotional feelings.


Cerebral Cortex | 2015

Auditory Feedback Differentially Modulates Behavioral and Neural Markers of Objective and Subjective Performance When Tapping to Your Heartbeat

Andrés Canales-Johnson; Carolina Silva; David Huepe; Álvaro Rivera-Rei; Valdas Noreika; María del Carmen García; Walter Silva; Carlos Ciraolo; Esteban Vaucheret; Lucas Sedeño; Blas Couto; Lucila Kargieman; Fabricio Baglivo; Mariano Sigman; Srivas Chennu; Agustín Ibáñez; Eugenio Rodriguez; Tristan A. Bekinschtein

Interoception, the perception of our body internal signals, plays a key role in maintaining homeostasis and guiding our behavior. Sometimes, we become aware of our body signals and use them in planning and strategic thinking. Here, we show behavioral and neural dissociations between learning to follow ones own heartbeat and metacognitive awareness of ones performance, in a heartbeat-tapping task performed before and after auditory feedback. The electroencephalography amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked potential in interoceptive learners, that is, participants whose accuracy of tapping to their heartbeat improved after auditory feedback, was higher compared with non-learners. However, an increase in gamma phase synchrony (30–45 Hz) after the heartbeat auditory feedback was present only in those participants showing agreement between objective interoceptive performance and metacognitive awareness. Source localization in a group of participants and direct cortical recordings in a single patient identified a network hub for interoceptive learning in the insular cortex. In summary, interoceptive learning may be mediated by the right insular response to the heartbeat, whereas metacognitive awareness of learning may be mediated by widespread cortical synchronization patterns.


Behavioral and Brain Functions | 2013

Preliminary evidence about the effects of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity and social cognition

Margherita Melloni; Lucas Sedeño; Blas Couto; Martin Reynoso; Carlos Gelormini; Roberto Favaloro; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Mariano Sigman; Facundo Manes; Agustín Ibáñez

BackgroundInteroception refers to the conscious perception of body signals. Mindfulness is a meditation practice that encourages individuals to focus on their internal experiences such as bodily sensations, thoughts, and emotions. In this study, we selected a behavioral measure of interoceptive sensitivity (heartbeat detection task, HBD) to compare the effect of meditation practice on interoceptive sensitivity among long term practitioners (LTP), short term meditators (STM, subjects that completed a Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program) and controls (non-meditators). All participants were examined with a battery of different tasks including mood state, executive function and social cognition tests (emotion recognition, empathy and theory of mind).FindingsCompared to controls, both meditators’ groups showed lower levels of anxiety and depression, but no improvement in executive function or social cognition performance was observed (except for lower scores compared to controls only in the personal distress dimension of empathy). More importantly, meditators’ performance did not differ from that of nonmeditators regarding cardiac interoceptive sensitivity.ConclusionResults suggest no influence of meditation practice in cardiac interoception and in most related social cognition measures. These negative results could be partially due to the fact that awareness of heartbeat sensations is not emphasized during mindfulness/vipassana meditation and may not be the best index of the awareness supported by the practice of meditation.


Autonomic Neuroscience: Basic and Clinical | 2015

Heart evoked potential triggers brain responses to natural affective scenes: A preliminary study.

Blas Couto; Federico Adolfi; María Velasquez; Marie Mesow; Justin S. Feinstein; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Ezequiel Mikulan; David Martínez-Pernía; Tristan A. Bekinschtein; Mariano Sigman; Facundo Manes; Agustín Ibáñez

The relationship between ongoing brain interoceptive signals and emotional processes has been addressed only indirectly through external stimulus-locked measures. In this study, an internal body trigger (heart evoked potential, HEP) was used to measure ongoing internally triggered signals during emotional states. We employed high-density electroencephalography (hd-EEG), source reconstruction analysis, and behavioral measures to assess healthy participants watching emotion-inducing video-clips (positive, negative, and neutral emotions). Results showed emotional modulation of the HEP at specific source-space nodes of the fronto-insulo-temporal networks related to affective-cognitive integration. This study is the first to assess the direct convergence among continuous triggers of viscerosensory cortical markers and emotion through dynamic stimuli presentation.


PLOS ONE | 2011

Fluid Intelligence and Psychosocial Outcome: From Logical Problem Solving to Social Adaptation

David Huepe; María Roca; Natalia Salas; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Álvaro Rivera-Rei; Leandro Zamorano; Aimée Concepción; Facundo Manes; Agustín Ibáñez

Background While fluid intelligence has proved to be central to executive functioning, logical reasoning and other frontal functions, the role of this ability in psychosocial adaptation has not been well characterized. Methodology/Principal Findings A random-probabilistic sample of 2370 secondary school students completed measures of fluid intelligence (Ravens Progressive Matrices, RPM) and several measures of psychological adaptation: bullying (Delaware Bullying Questionnaire), domestic abuse of adolescents (Conflict Tactic Scale), drug intake (ONUDD), self-esteem (Rosenbergs Self Esteem Scale) and the Perceived Mental Health Scale (Spanish adaptation). Lower fluid intelligence scores were associated with physical violence, both in the role of victim and victimizer. Drug intake, especially cannabis, cocaine and inhalants and lower self-esteem were also associated with lower fluid intelligence. Finally, scores on the perceived mental health assessment were better when fluid intelligence scores were higher. Conclusions/Significance Our results show evidence of a strong association between psychosocial adaptation and fluid intelligence, suggesting that the latter is not only central to executive functioning but also forms part of a more general capacity for adaptation to social contexts.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2015

Disentangling interoception: insights from focal strokes affecting the perception of external and internal milieus.

Blas Couto; Federico Adolfi; Lucas Sedeño; Alejo Salles; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Pablo Alvarez-Abut; Indira García-Cordero; Marcos Pietto; Tristan A. Bekinschtein; Mariano Sigman; Facundo Manes; Agustín Ibáñez

Interoception is the moment-to-moment sensing of the physiological condition of the body. The multimodal sources of interoception can be classified into two different streams of afferents: an internal pathway of signals arising from core structures (i.e., heart, blood vessels, and bronchi) and an external pathway of body-mapped sensations (i.e., chemosensation and pain) arising from peripersonal space. This study examines differential processing along these streams within the insular cortex (IC) and their subcortical tracts connecting frontotemporal networks. Two rare patients presenting focal lesions of the IC (insular lesion, IL) or its subcortical tracts (subcortical lesion, SL) were tested. Internally generated interoceptive streams were assessed through a heartbeat detection (HBD) task, while those externally triggered were tapped via taste, smell, and pain recognition tasks. A differential pattern was observed. The IC patient showed impaired internal signal processing while the SL patient exhibited external perception deficits. Such selective deficits remained even when comparing each patient with a group of healthy controls and a group of brain-damaged patients. These outcomes suggest the existence of distinguishable interoceptive streams. Results are discussed in relation with neuroanatomical substrates, involving a fronto-insulo-temporal network for interoceptive and cognitive contextual integration.


PLOS ONE | 2013

Attachment Patterns Trigger Differential Neural Signature of Emotional Processing in Adolescents

María Josefina Escobar; Álvaro Rivera-Rei; Jean Decety; David Huepe; Juan Felipe Cardona; Andrés Canales-Johnson; Mariano Sigman; Ezequiel Mikulan; Elena Helgiu; Sandra Baez; Facundo Manes; Vladimir López; Agustín Ibáñez

Background Research suggests that individuals with different attachment patterns process social information differently, especially in terms of facial emotion recognition. However, few studies have explored social information processes in adolescents. This study examined the behavioral and ERP correlates of emotional processing in adolescents with different attachment orientations (insecure attachment group and secure attachment group; IAG and SAG, respectively). This study also explored the association of these correlates to individual neuropsychological profiles. Methodology/Principal Findings We used a modified version of the dual valence task (DVT), in which participants classify stimuli (faces and words) according to emotional valence (positive or negative). Results showed that the IAG performed significantly worse than SAG on tests of executive function (EF attention, processing speed, visuospatial abilities and cognitive flexibility). In the behavioral DVT, the IAG presented lower performance and accuracy. The IAG also exhibited slower RTs for stimuli with negative valence. Compared to the SAG, the IAG showed a negative bias for faces; a larger P1 and attenuated N170 component over the right hemisphere was observed. A negative bias was also observed in the IAG for word stimuli, which was demonstrated by comparing the N170 amplitude of the IAG with the valence of the SAG. Finally, the amplitude of the N170 elicited by the facial stimuli correlated with EF in both groups (and negative valence with EF in the IAG). Conclusions/Significance Our results suggest that individuals with different attachment patterns process key emotional information and corresponding EF differently. This is evidenced by an early modulation of ERP components’ amplitudes, which are correlated with behavioral and neuropsychological effects. In brief, attachments patterns appear to impact multiple domains, such as emotional processing and EFs.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2015

Neurophenomenology revisited: second-person methods for the study of human consciousness.

Francisco Olivares; Esteban Vargas; Claudio Fuentes; David Martínez-Pernía; Andrés Canales-Johnson

In the study of consciousness, neurophenomenology was originally established as a novel research program attempting to reconcile two apparently irreconcilable methodologies in psychology: qualitative and quantitative methods. Its potential relies on Francisco Varela’s idea of reciprocal constraints, in which first-person accounts and neurophysiological data mutually inform each other. However, since its first conceptualization, neurophenomenology has encountered methodological problems. These problems have emerged mainly because of the difficulty of obtaining and analyzing subjective reports in a systematic manner. However, more recently, several interview techniques for describing subjective accounts have been developed, collectively known as “second-person methods.” Second-person methods refer to interview techniques that solicit both verbal and non-verbal information from participants in order to obtain systematic and detailed subjective reports. Here, we examine the potential for employing second-person methodologies in the neurophenomenological study of consciousness and we propose three practical ideas for developing a second-person neurophenomenological method. Thus, we first describe second-person methodologies available in the literature for analyzing subjective reports, identifying specific constraints on the status of the first-, second- and third- person methods. Second, we analyze two experimental studies that explicitly incorporate second-person methods for traversing the “gap” between phenomenology and neuroscience. Third, we analyze the challenges that second-person accounts face in establishing an objective methodology for comparing results across different participants and interviewers: this is the “validation” problem. Finally, we synthesize the common aspects of the interview methods described above. In conclusion, our arguments emphasize that second-person methods represent a powerful approach for closing the gap between the experiential and the neurobiological levels of description in the study of human consciousness.


bioRxiv | 2017

Wakefulness state modulates conscious access: Suppression of auditory detection in the transition to sleep

Valdas Noreika; Andrés Canales-Johnson; William J. Harrison; Amy Johnson; Aurina Arnatkevičiūtė; Justin Koh; Srivas Chennu; Tristan A. Bekinschtein

Neural basis of consciousness and its suppression are typically studied by manipulating stimuli around the conscious access threshold, or — alternatively — by contrasting conscious and unconscious states (i.e. awake/sleep). Here we show that behavioural and neural markers of conscious access are dependent on wakefulness state, and thus a comprehensive description of the neural basis of conscious access requires an integrated assessment of the state of consciousness. In particular, we demonstrate that a distinctive steepness of a behavioural slope of conscious access is severely compromised during the transition to sleep. Likewise, electrophysiological markers show a delayed processing of target-mask interaction during drowsiness. Consequently, the resolution of conscious access shifts from perceptual to executive stages of processing in the drowsy state of consciousness. Once the goal is set — to report the awareness of a target — the brain is capable to adapt to rapidly changing wakefulness states, revealing that the neural signatures of conscious access and its suppression may not be hard-wired but flexible to maintain performance.Mapping the reports of awareness and its neural underpinnings is instrumental to understand the limits of human perception. The capacity to become aware of objects in the world may be studied by suppressing faint target stimuli with strong masking stimuli, or – alternatively – by manipulating the level of wakefulness from full alertness to mild drowsiness. By combining these two approaches, we studied how perceptual awareness is modulated by decreasing wakefulness. We found dynamic changes in behavioural and neural signatures of conscious access in humans between awake and drowsy states. Behaviourally, we show a decrease in the steepness of the psychophysical function for conscious access in drowsy trials. Neural mapping showed delayed processing of target-mask interaction as the consciousness transition progressed, suggesting that the brain resolution of conscious access shifts from early sensory/perceptual to decision-making stages of processing. Once the goal to report the awareness of a target is set, the system behaviourally adapts to rapid changes in wakefulness, revealing the flexibility of the neural signatures of conscious access, and its suppression, to maintain performance. Significance statement Maintaining full alertness for long periods of time in attentionally demanding situations is challenging and may lead to a decrease in performance. We show the effect of wakefulness fluctuations on behaviour and brain dynamics that humans use to maintain performance. We reveal the neural strategies we have to cope with drowsiness by shifting the weights to more flexible brain processes and relaxing the precision of the decisions we take.

Collaboration


Dive into the Andrés Canales-Johnson's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Facundo Manes

National Scientific and Technical Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mariano Sigman

Torcuato di Tella University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David Huepe

Diego Portales University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge