Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Anna Scandurra is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Anna Scandurra.


Animal Cognition | 2016

Ontogenetic effects on gazing behaviour: a case study of kennel dogs (Labrador Retrievers) in the impossible task paradigm

Anna Scandurra

Life experiences and living conditions can influence the problem-solving strategies and the communicative abilities of dogs with humans. The goals of this study were to determine any behavioural differences between Labrador Retrievers living in a kennel and those living in a house as pets and to assess whether kennel dogs show preferences in social behaviours for their caretaker relative to a stranger when they are faced with an unsolvable task. Nine Labrador Retrievers living in a kennel from birth and ten Labrador Retrievers living in a family as pets were tested. The experimental procedure consisted of three “solvable” tasks in which the dogs could easily retrieve food from a container followed by an “unsolvable” task in which the container was hermetically locked. Dogs of both groups spent the same amount of time interacting with the experimental apparatus. Kennel dogs gazed towards people for less time and with higher latency than pet dogs; however, there were no significant preferences in gazing towards the stranger versus the caretaker in both groups. These findings demonstrated that kennel dogs are less prone to use human-directed gazing behaviour when they are faced with an unsolvable problem, taking the humans into account to solve a task less than do the pet dogs.


Animal Cognition | 2016

The importance of gestural communication: a study of human–dog communication using incongruent information

Biagio D’Aniello; Anna Scandurra; Alessandra Alterisio; Paola Valsecchi; Emanuela Prato-Previde

We assessed how water rescue dogs, which were equally accustomed to respond to gestural and verbal requests, weighted gestural versus verbal information when asked by their owner to perform an action. Dogs were asked to perform four different actions (“sit”, “lie down”, “stay”, “come”) providing them with a single source of information (in Phase 1, gestural, and in Phase 2, verbal) or with incongruent information (in Phase 3, gestural and verbal commands referred to two different actions). In Phases 1 and 2, we recorded the frequency of correct responses as 0 or 1, whereas in Phase 3, we computed a ‘preference index’ (percentage of gestural commands followed over the total commands responded). Results showed that dogs followed gestures significantly better than words when these two types of information were used separately. Females were more likely to respond to gestural than verbal commands and males responded to verbal commands significantly better than females. In the incongruent condition, when gestures and words simultaneously indicated two different actions, the dogs overall preferred to execute the action required by the gesture rather than that required verbally, except when the verbal command “come” was paired with the gestural command “stay” with the owner moving away from the dog. Our data suggest that in dogs accustomed to respond to both gestural and verbal requests, gestures are more salient than words. However, dogs’ responses appeared to be dependent also on the contextual situation: dogs’ motivation to maintain proximity with an owner who was moving away could have led them to make the more ‘convenient’ choices between the two incongruent instructions.


Animal Cognition | 2017

What’s the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand human pointing gestures

Biagio D’Aniello; Alessandra Alterisio; Anna Scandurra; Emanuele Petremolo; Maria Rosaria Iommelli; Massimo Aria

In many studies that have investigated whether dogs’ capacities to understand human pointing gestures are aspects of evolutionary or developmental social competences, family-owned dogs have been compared to shelter dogs. However, for most of these studies, the origins of shelter dogs were unknown. Some shelter dogs may have lived with families before entering shelters, and from these past experiences, they may have learned to understand human gestures. Furthermore, there is substantial variation in the methodology and analytic approaches used in such studies (e.g. different pointing protocols, different treatment of trials with no-choice response and indoor vs. outdoor experimental arenas). Such differences in methodologies and analysis techniques used make it difficult to compare results obtained from different studies and may account for the divergent results obtained. We thus attempted to control for several parameters by carrying out a test on dynamic proximal and distal pointing. We studied eleven kennel dogs of known origin that were born and raised in a kennels with limited human interaction. This group was compared to a group of eleven dogs comparable in terms of breed, sex and age that had lived with human families since they were puppies. Our results demonstrate that pet dogs outperform kennel dogs in their comprehension of proximal and distal pointing, regardless of whether trials where no-choice was made were considered as errors or were excluded from statistical analysis, meaning that dogs living in kennels do not understand pointing gestures. Even if genetic effects of the domestication process on human–dog relationships cannot be considered as negligible, our data suggest that dogs need to learn human pointing gestures and thus underscore the importance of ontogenetic processes.


Animal Cognition | 2018

Interspecies transmission of emotional information via chemosignals: from humans to dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)

Biagio D’Aniello; Gün Refik Semin; Alessandra Alterisio; Massimo Aria; Anna Scandurra

We report a study examining interspecies emotion transfer via body odors (chemosignals). Do human body odors (chemosignals) produced under emotional conditions of happiness and fear provide information that is detectable by pet dogs (Labrador and Golden retrievers)? The odor samples were collected from the axilla of male donors not involved in the main experiment. The experimental setup involved the co-presence of the dog’s owner, a stranger and the odor dispenser in a space where the dogs could move freely. There were three odor conditions [fear, happiness, and control (no sweat)] to which the dogs were assigned randomly. The dependent variables were the relevant behaviors of the dogs (e.g., approaching, interacting and gazing) directed to the three targets (owner, stranger, sweat dispenser) aside from the dogs’ stress and heart rate indicators. The results indicated with high accuracy that the dogs manifested the predicted behaviors in the three conditions. There were fewer and shorter owner directed behaviors and more stranger directed behaviors when they were in the “happy odor condition” compared to the fear odor and control conditions. In the fear odor condition, they displayed more stressful behaviors. The heart rate data in the control and happy conditions were significantly lower than in the fear condition. Our findings suggest that interspecies emotional communication is facilitated by chemosignals.


Cell and Tissue Research | 2016

Neuroanatomical relationships between FMRFamide- immunoreactive components of the nervus terminalis and the topology of olfactory bulbs in teleost fish

Biagio D’Aniello; Gianluca Polese; Luciano Luongo; Anna Scandurra; Laura Magliozzi; Massimo Aria; Claudia Pinelli

The nervus terminalis (NT) is the most anterior of the vertebrate cranial nerves. In teleost fish, the NT runs across all olfactory components and shows high morphological variability within this taxon. We compare the anatomical distribution, average number and size of the FMRFamide-immunoreactive (ir) NT cells of fourteen teleost species with different positions of olfactory bulbs (OBs) with respect to the ventral telencephalic area. Based on the topology of the OBs, three different neuroanatomical organizations of the telencephalon can be defined, viz., fish having sessile (Type I), pseudosessile (short stalked; Type II) or stalked (Type III) OBs. Type III topology of OBs appears to be a feature associated with more basal species, whereas Types I and II occur in derived and in basal species. The displacement of the OBs is positively correlated with the peripheral distribution of the FMRFamide-ir NT cells. The number of cells is negatively correlated with the size of the cells. A dependence analysis related to the type of OB topology revealed a positive relationship with the number of cells and with the size of the cells, with Type I and II topologies of OBs showing significantly fewer cells and larger cells than Type III. A dendrogram based on similarities obtained by taking into account all variables under study, i.e., the number and size of the FMRFamide-ir NT cells and the topology of OBs, does not agree with the phylogenetic relationships amongst species, suggesting that divergent or convergent evolutionary phenomena produced the olfactory components studied.


Neuroscience Letters | 2017

d-Aspartic acid ameliorates painful and neuropsychiatric changes and reduces β-amyloid Aβ1-42 peptide in a long lasting model of neuropathic pain

Antimo D’Aniello; Livio Luongo; Rosaria Romano; Monica Iannotta; Ida Marabese; Serena Boccella; Carmela Belardo; Vito de Novellis; Claudio Arra; Antonio Barbieri; Biagio D’Aniello; Anna Scandurra; Laura Magliozzi; George H. Fisher; Francesca Guida; Sabatino Maione

Depressive symptoms and other neuropsychiatric dysfunctions are common in neurodegenerative disorders, including chronic pain and dementia. A correlation between the β-amyloid protein accumulation and the development of depression has been suggested, however the underlying mechanisms are unknown. d-Aspartate (d-Asp) is a free d-amino acid found in the mammalian brain and involved in neurological and psychiatric processes, such as cognition and affective disorders. In this study we have investigated the effects of a repeated treatment with d-Asp in a long-lasting (12 months) model of neuropathic pain, the spared nerve injury (SNI), in mice. Specifically, we evaluated i) the pain sensitivity and related emotional/cognitive dysfunctions induced by SNI, ii) possible changes in the β-amyloid protein accumulation in specific brain regions involved in pain mechanisms ii) possible changes in steroids level in neuropathic animals with or without d-Asp in the same brain areas. SNI mice showed an increase of the insoluble form of Aβ1-42 at hippocampal level and displayed cognitive impairments, stereotypical and depressive-like behaviours. d-Asp treatment reduced abnormal behaviours and normalized the β-amyloid protein expression. Moreover, d-Asp dramatically increased steroids level measured in the prefrontal cortex and in the hippocampus. Our findings provide new insights into pain mechanisms and suggest a possible role of β-amyloid protein in neuropsychiatric dysfunctions associated with chronic pain.


Animal Cognition | 2017

Recognition of human faces by dogs (Canis familiaris) requires visibility of head contour

Paolo Mongillo; Anna Scandurra; Robin S. S. Kramer; Lieta Marinelli

Researchers have suggested that dogs are able to recognise human faces, but conclusive evidence has yet to be found. Experiment 1 of this study investigated whether dogs can recognise humans using visual information from the face/head region, and whether this also occurs in conditions of suboptimal visibility of the face. Dogs were presented with their owner’s and a stranger’s heads, protruding through openings of an apparatus in opposite parts of the experimental setting. Presentations occurred in conditions of either optimal or suboptimal visibility; the latter featured non-frontal orientation, uneven illumination and invisibility of outer contours of the heads. Instances where dogs approached their owners with a higher frequency than predicted by chance were considered evidence of recognition. This occurred only in the optimal condition. With a similar paradigm, Experiment 2 investigated which of the alterations in visibility that characterised the suboptimal condition accounted for dogs’ inability to recognise owners. Dogs approached their owners more frequently than predicted by chance if outer head contours were visible, but not if heads were either frontally oriented or evenly illuminated. Moreover, male dogs were slightly better at recognition than females. These findings represent the first clear demonstration that dogs can recognise human faces and that outer face elements are crucial for such a task, complementing previous research on human face processing in dogs. Parallels with face recognition abilities observed in other animal species, as well as with human infants, point to the relevance of these results from a comparative standpoint.


Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy | 2018

Distribution of NADPH-diaphorase reactivity in the central nervous system of the common toad ( Bufo bufo )

Claudia Pinelli; Arun G. Jadhao; Anna Scandurra; Biagio D’Aniello

We examined the distribution of nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase (NADPH-d)-reactive elements in the central nervous system (CNS) of the common toad, Bufo bufo. The investigation involved adult male and female toads collected during the breeding season. Labeled neurons of different morphological appearances (weakly or darkly stained, unipolar, bipolar, and multipolar) and fibers were observed across all subdivisions of the amphibian brain. Overall, a similar distribution of NADPH-d-labeled neurons was observed in the brain of male and female toads. In the secondary prosencephalon NADPH-d-labeled neurons were observed in the olfactory bulbs, pallial regions, nucleus accumbens, diagonal band of Broca, septum, striatum, amygdala, suprachiasmatic and magnocellular preoptic nuclei, dorsal and ventral hypothalamus. In the diencephalon, NADPH-d-positive neurons were seen in the anterior thalamic nuclei, ventromedial and ventrolateral nuclei, central and lateral thalamic nuclei, posterior tubercle, posterodorsal division of the lateral thalamic nucleus, and in the pretectal and pretoral gray. In the mesencephalon, heavily stained neurons were present in the anterodorsal and anteroventral tegmental nuclei, magnocellular, principal and laminar nuclei of the torus semicircularis, and nucleus profundus mesencephali. In the isthmus, stained cells were observed medially and ventrally in the posterodorsal and posteroventral tegmental nuclei. In the rhombencephalon, numerous NADPH-d-stained neurons were distributed in the cerebellar nucleus, sensory and descending trigeminal nuclei, motor nuclei of the glossopharyngeal and vagus nerves, the nucleus of the solitary tract, nuclei of the hypoglossal and octaval nerves, dorsal column nucleus, central gray region, and in reticular formation. However, the complete absence of NADPH-d-stained neurons in the cerebellar cortex was an unusual feature observed in this study. The widespread distribution of NADPH-d staining in diverse cell types, belonging to a variety of neuronal systems suggests a widespread role for NADPH-d in modulating diverse functions, including sensory coding in the amphibian nervous system.


Journal of Experimental Zoology | 2016

Proliferative events and apoptotic remodelling in retinal development of common toad (Bufo bufo)

Claudia Pinelli; Alfredo Sansone; Anna De Maio; Antonietta Morgillo; Anna Scandurra

Proliferation and apoptosis are fundamental processes in the development of the retina, and a proper balance of the two phenomena is crucial to correct development of the organ. Despite intense investigation in different vertebrates, only a few studies have analyzed the cell death and the cell division quantitatively in the same species during development. Here we studied the time course of apoptosis and proliferation in the retina of common toad, Bufo bufo, and discuss the findings in an evolutionary perspective. We found cells that were dividing first scattered throughout the retina, then, in later stages, proliferation was confined to the ciliary marginal zone. This pattern was confirmed by the expression of the proliferative marker PCNA. Both proliferation and apoptosis occurred in successive waves, and two apoptotic peaks were detected: one at premetamorphosis 1 and the second at prometamorphosis. PARP-1, a known molecular marker of apoptosis, was used to confirm the data obtained by counting pyknotic nuclei. In summary, proliferative and apoptotic waves display an inverse time-relationship through development, with apoptotic peaks coinciding with low proliferation phases. In a comparative perspective, amphibians follow a developmental pattern similar to other vertebrates, although with different timing.


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2017

Effectiveness of verbal and gestural signals and familiarity with signal-senders on the performance of working dogs

Anna Scandurra; Alessandra Alterisio; Lieta Marinelli; Paolo Mongillo; Gün Refik Semin; Biagio D’Aniello

Collaboration


Dive into the Anna Scandurra's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Biagio D’Aniello

University of Naples Federico II

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alessandra Alterisio

University of Naples Federico II

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Claudia Pinelli

Seconda Università degli Studi di Napoli

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Massimo Aria

University of Naples Federico II

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anna De Maio

University of Naples Federico II

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Antimo D’Aniello

Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Antonietta Morgillo

University of Naples Federico II

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge