Antony W. Goodwin
University of Melbourne
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Antony W. Goodwin.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 2004
Heather E. Wheat; Lauren M. Salo; Antony W. Goodwin
When humans manipulate objects, the sensorimotor system coordinates three-dimensional forces to optimize and maintain grasp stability. To do this, the CNS requires precise information about the magnitude and direction of load force (tangential to skin surface) plus feedback about grip force (normal to skin). Previous studies have shown that there is rapid, precise coordination between grip and load forces that deteriorates with digital nerve block. Obviously, mechanoreceptive afferents innervating fingerpad skin contribute essential information. We quantify human capacity to scale tangential and normal forces using only cutaneous information. Our paradigm simulated natural manipulations (a force tangential to the skin superimposed on an indenting force normal to the skin). Precisely controlled forces were applied by a custom-built stimulator to an immobilized fingerpad. Using magnitude estimation, subjects (n = 8) scaled the magnitude of tangential force (0.25–2.8 N) in two experiments (normal force, 2.5 and 4 N, respectively). Performance was unaffected by normal force magnitude and tangential force direction. Moreover, when both normal (2–4 N) and tangential forces were varied in a randomized-block factorial design, the relationship between applied and perceived tangential force remained near linear, with a minor but statistically significant nonlinearity. Our subjects could also discriminate small differences in tangential force, and this was the case for two different reference stimuli. In both cases, the Weber fraction was 0.16. Finally, scaling functions for magnitude estimates of normal force (1–5 N) were also approximately linear. These data show that the cutaneous afferents provide a wealth of precise information about both normal and tangential force.
The Journal of Physiology | 2010
Ingvars Birznieks; Heather E. Wheat; Stephen J. Redmond; Lauren M. Salo; Nigel H. Lovell; Antony W. Goodwin
Torsional loads are ubiquitous during everyday dextrous manipulations. We examined how information about torque is provided to the sensorimotor control system by populations of tactile afferents. Torsional loads of different magnitudes were applied in clockwise and anticlockwise directions to a standard central site on the fingertip. Three different background levels of contact (grip) force were used. The median nerve was exposed in anaesthetized monkeys and single unit responses recorded from 66 slowly adapting type‐I (SA‐I) and 31 fast adapting type‐I (FA‐I) afferents innervating the distal segments of the fingertips. Most afferents were excited by torque but some were suppressed. Responses of the majority of both afferent types were scaled by torque magnitude applied in one or other direction, with the majority of FA‐I afferent responses and about half of SA‐I afferent responses scaled in both directions. Torque direction affected responses in both afferent types, but more so for the SA‐I afferents. Latencies of the first spike in FA‐I afferent responses depended on the parameters of the torque. We used a Parzen window classifier to assess the capacity of the SA‐I and FA‐I afferent populations to discriminate, concurrently and in real‐time, the three stimulus parameters, namely background normal force, torque magnitude and direction. Despite the potentially confounding interactions between stimulus parameters, both the SA‐I and the FA‐I populations could extract torque magnitude accurately. The FA‐I afferents signalled torque magnitude earlier than did the SA‐I afferents, but torque direction was extracted more rapidly and more accurately by the SA‐I afferent population.
Journal of Neurophysiology | 2010
Heather E. Wheat; Lauren M. Salo; Antony W. Goodwin
Control of tangential force plays a key role in everyday manipulations. In anesthetized monkeys, forces tangential to the skin were applied at a range of magnitudes comparable to those used in routine manipulations and in eight different directions. The paradigm used enabled separation of responses to tangential force from responses to the background normal force. For slowly adapting type I (SAI) afferents, tangential force responses ranged from excitatory through no response to suppression, with both a static and dynamic component. For fast adapting type I (FAI) afferents, responses were dynamic and excitatory only. Responses of both afferent types were scaled by tangential force magnitude, elucidating the neural basis for previous human psychophysical scaling data. Most afferents were direction selective with a range of preferred directions and a range in sharpness of tuning. Both the preferred direction and the degree of tuning were independent of the background normal force. Preferred directions were distributed uniformly over 360 degrees for SAI afferents, but for FAI afferents they were biased toward the proximo-ulnar direction. Afferents from all over the glabrous skin of the distal segment of the finger responded; there was no evident relationship between the position of an afferents receptive field on the finger and its preferred direction or its degree of tuning. Nor were preferred directions biased either toward or away from the receptive field center. In response to the relatively large normal forces, some afferents saturated and others did not, regardless of the positions of their receptive fields. Total afferent response matched human psychophysical scaling functions for normal force.
Archive | 2008
Antony W. Goodwin; Heather E. Wheat
When we manipulate objects in our environment, a vast array of receptors in the skin, joints and muscles is activated. This information is relayed to the central nervous system and underlies two distinct but complementary aspects of hand function. Most obviously, these neural signals lead to haptic perception. We may sense how rough or smooth a surface is, or how curved an object is, whether it is soft or hard, whether the surface is slippery or sticky, how heavy it is and so on. Less obvious, but equally important, is the use the motor control system makes of these sensory signals in order to ensure appropriate hand movements resulting in stable grasps and effective complex manipulations. Some examples of common manipulations in our daily lives are: lifting a cup of coffee, opening a door, getting dressed, typing a manuscript, threading a needle.
international conference on human haptic sensing and touch enabled computer applications | 2010
Stephen J. Redmond; Ingvars Birznieks; Nigel H. Lovell; Antony W. Goodwin
In this study, tactile afferents in monkey fingertips were mechanically stimulated, using a flat disc shaped probe, with several magnitudes of torque, in clockwise and anticlockwise directions. In order to prevent slip during the stimulation event, a sufficient normal force was also applied, with three different magnitudes tested. Recordings were made from afferents innervating the glabrous skin covering the entire distal segment of the finger. A Parzen window classifier was used to assess the capacity of tactile afferents to discriminate, concurrently and in real-time, the three stimulus parameters; namely, background normal force, torque magnitude and direction. Despite the potentially confounding interactions between stimulus parameters, classification accuracy was very high and was improved even further by selecting subsets of best performing afferents.
Current Biology | 1993
Antony W. Goodwin
Abstract Comparing psychophysical measurement in humans with neural recordings in monkeys points to a neural code for tactile roughness.
Archive | 1996
Antony W. Goodwin; As Browning; Heather E. Wheat
Everyday, humans grasp and manipulate objects with precision. In order to achieve this, the neural mechanisms controlling movements of the fingers must have continuous information about the relationships of the fingers to the object being handled. In particular, successful manipulations depend on a knowledge of the local shape of the object, its position on the fingers and the contact forces. Some of this information is relayed by receptors in the joints and in the muscles (Burgess et al., 1982), and joint angle is also signalled by cutaneous receptors around the joints and in the hairy skin of the hand (Edin and Abbs, 1991). The fingerpads are densely innervated with low threshold mechanoreceptors which signal a variety of precise information about grasp (Johansson and Vallbo, 1979). They are the only source of information about the region of contact with the fingers, an important consideration from the point of view of the dynamics of grasp (Fearing and Hollerbach, 1985). Receptors in the extrinsic and intrinsic hand muscles signal forces used by the fingers but information about contact forces between the fingers and the object is also signalled by the cutaneous receptors in the fingerpads (Cohen and Vierck, 1993)
international conference of the ieee engineering in medicine and biology society | 2012
Joanne Fu; Ingvars Birznieks; Antony W. Goodwin; Heba Khamis; Stephen J. Redmond
How complex tactile sensations are encoded by populations of afferent mechanoreceptors is currently not well understood. While much is known about how individual afferents respond to prescribed stimuli, their behavior as a population distributed across the fingertip has not been well described. In this study, tactile afferent mechanoreceptors in monkey fingertips were mechanically stimulated, using a flat disc shaped probe, with several magnitudes of normal force (1.8, 2.2 and 2.5 N) and torque (2.0 and 3.5 mNm), in clockwise and anticlockwise directions. Afferent nerve responses were acquired from 58 slowly-adapting (SA) type-I and 25 fast-adapting (FA) type-I isolated single cutaneous mechanoreceptive afferents, recorded from the median nerve. At 10 ms time intervals after the application of torque begins, a multiple regression model was trained and evaluated to estimate the magnitude of the applied normal force and torque. Averaged results over the 200 ms period after the torque reaches its maximum indicate that SA-I and FA-I afferents can both estimate the applied torque value. FA-I afferents gave the lowest estimation error mean and standard deviation of -0.051 ± 0.334 mNm for a target torque of 2.0 mNm, and 0.003 ± 0.414 mNm for a target torque of 3.5 mNm. However, while SA-I afferents could estimate normal force well, there was no significant difference (ANOVA, p=0.173) in the FA-I estimates of normal force, as this force had already been held constant for one second before the torque loading phase under analysis began.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods | 1983
Ian Darian-Smith; Geoffrey Durham-Smith; Michio Sugitani; John Heywood; Antony W. Goodwin
A new method is described for recording responses of single neurons in the monkeys cerebral cortex by passing a microelectrode directly through the overlying skin and dura. Previous to this recording a window had been cut in the calvarium, and the bone deficit repaired using a full-thickness skin graft. Stable unitary recordings have been made over a period of one year following craniotomy, the only skull attachment being a small stainless steel peg, on which the microdrive was mounted when required.
The Journal of Neuroscience | 2001
Ingvars Birznieks; Per Jenmalm; Antony W. Goodwin; Roland S. Johansson