Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Asako Yasuda is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Asako Yasuda.


Cognition | 2005

Illusion of sense of self-agency: discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of actions modulates the sense of self-agency, but not the sense of self-ownership

Atsushi Sato; Asako Yasuda

It is proposed that knowledge of motor commands is used to distinguish self-generated sensation from externally generated sensation. In this paper, we show that the sense of self-agency, that is the sense that I am the one who is generating an action, largely depends on the degree of discrepancy resulting from comparison between the predicted and actual sensory feedback. In Experiment 1, the sense of self-agency was reduced when the presentation of the tone was unpredictable in terms of timing and its frequency, although in fact the tone was self-produced. In Experiment 2, the opposite case was found to occur. That is, participants experienced illusionary sense of self-agency when the externally generated sensations happened to match the prediction made by forward model. In Experiment 3, the sense of self-agency was reduced when there was a discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences, regardless of presence or absence of a discrepancy between the intended and actual consequences of actions. In all the experiments, a discrepancy between the predicted and actual feedback had no effects on sense of self-ownership, that is the sense that I am the one who is undergoing an experience. These results may suggest that both senses of self are mutually independent.


Neuroreport | 2005

Effects of value and reward magnitude on feedback negativity and P300.

Atsushi Sato; Asako Yasuda; Hideki Ohira; Kaori Miyawaki; Masami Nishikawa; Hiroaki Kumano; Tomifusa Kuboki

Feedback negativity is a negative component of the event-related brain potential observed 250–300 ms after feedback stimuli. The present study investigated the effects of value (correct or incorrect) and reward magnitude (no, small or large) on feedback negativity and P300. Feedback negativity was larger after incorrect feedback than after correct feedback, irrespective of reward magnitude. In contrast, P300 amplitude increased with reward magnitude, irrespective of value. The amplitude of feedback negativity was correlated with a trait score of negative affect and not positive affect, whereas P300 amplitude was correlated with positive affect and not negative affect. These results suggest that value and reward magnitude are processed separately in the brain.


Neuroreport | 2005

Cerebral glucose metabolism associated with a fear network in panic disorder

Yojiro Sakai; Hiroaki Kumano; Masami Nishikawa; Yuji Sakano; Hisanobu Kaiya; Etsuko Imabayashi; Takashi Ohnishi; Hiroshi Matsuda; Asako Yasuda; Atsushi Sato; Mirko Diksic; Tomifusa Kuboki

The present study was performed to assess cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with panic disorder using positron emission tomography. 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography with voxel-based analysis was used to compare regional brain glucose utilization in 12 nonmedicated panic disorder patients, without their experiencing panic attacks during positron emission tomography acquisition, with that in 22 healthy controls. Panic disorder patients showed appreciably high state anxiety before scanning, and exhibited significantly higher levels of glucose uptake in the bilateral amygdala, hippocampus, and thalamus, and in the midbrain, caudal pons, medulla, and cerebellum than controls. These results provided the first functional neuroimaging support in human patients for the neuroanatomical hypothesis of panic disorder focusing on the amygdala-based fear network.


Neuroreport | 2004

Error-related negativity reflects detection of negative reward prediction error.

Asako Yasuda; Atsushi Sato; Kaori Miyawaki; Hiroaki Kumano; Tomifusa Kuboki

Error-related negativity (ERN) is a negative deflection in the event-related potential elicited in error trials. To examine the function of ERN, we performed an experiment in which two within-participants factors were manipulated: outcome uncertainty and content of feedback. The ERN was largest when participants expected correct feedback but received error feedback. There were significant positive correlations between the ERN amplitude and the rate of response switching in the subsequent trial, and between the ERN amplitude and the trait version score on negative affect scale. These results suggest that ERN reflects detection of a negative reward prediction error and promotes subsequent response switching, and that individuals with high negative affect are hypersensitive to a negative reward prediction error.


Bioelectromagnetics | 2009

Effects of short-term W-CDMA mobile phone base station exposure on women with or without mobile phone related symptoms

Toshiaki Furubayashi; Akira Ushiyama; Yasuo Terao; Yoko Mizuno; Kei Shirasawa; Pornanong Pongpaibool; Ally Y. Simba; Kanako Wake; Masami Nishikawa; Kaori Miyawaki; Asako Yasuda; Mitsunori Uchiyama; Hitomi Kobayashi Yamashita; Hiroshi Masuda; Shogo Hirota; Miyuki Takahashi; Tomoko Okano; Satomi Inomata-Terada; Shigeru Sokejima; Eiji Maruyama; Soichi Watanabe; Masao Taki; Chiyoji Ohkubo; Yoshikazu Ugawa

To investigate possible health effects of mobile phone use, we conducted a double-blind, cross-over provocation study to confirm whether subjects with mobile phone related symptoms (MPRS) are more susceptible than control subjects to the effect of electromagnetic fields (EMF) emitted from base stations. We sent questionnaires to 5,000 women and obtained 2,472 valid responses from possible candidates; from these, we recruited 11 subjects with MPRS and 43 controls. There were four EMF exposure conditions, each of which lasted 30 min: continuous, intermittent, and sham exposure with and without noise. Subjects were exposed to EMF of 2.14 GHz, 10 V/m (W-CDMA), in a shielded room to simulate whole-body exposure to EMF from base stations, although the exposure strength we used was higher than that commonly received from base stations. We measured several psychological and cognitive parameters pre- and post-exposure, and monitored autonomic functions. Subjects were asked to report on their perception of EMF and level of discomfort during the experiment. The MPRS group did not differ from the controls in their ability to detect exposure to EMF; nevertheless they consistently experienced more discomfort, regardless of whether or not they were actually exposed to EMF, and despite the lack of significant changes in their autonomic functions. Thus, the two groups did not differ in their responses to real or sham EMF exposure according to any psychological, cognitive or autonomic assessment. In conclusion, we found no evidence of any causal link between hypersensitivity symptoms and exposure to EMF from base stations.


Neuroreport | 2005

Explicit knowledge and intention to learn in sequence learning: an event-related potential study

Kaori Miyawaki; Atsushi Sato; Asako Yasuda; Hiroaki Kumano; Tomifusa Kuboki

The present study was performed to examine how intention to learn and explicit knowledge in sequence learning are reflected in event-related potentials. Participants responded to numerals presented in a repeating order, which were replaced infrequently by deviant numerals. The participants were given incidental or intentional learning instructions. Sequence parts for which they acquired explicit knowledge were identified for each participant by post-task memory tests. Reaction times indicated that sequence learning occurred under both types of instruction. The N2 enhancement for deviants was primarily associated with explicit sequence knowledge, and the P3 enhancement showed a weak association. These results suggested that N2 and P3 reflect different aspects of explicit learning.


NeuroImage | 2006

Changes in cerebral glucose utilization in patients with panic disorder treated with cognitive-behavioral therapy

Yojiro Sakai; Hiroaki Kumano; Masami Nishikawa; Yuji Sakano; Hisanobu Kaiya; Etsuko Imabayashi; Takashi Ohnishi; Hiroshi Matsuda; Asako Yasuda; Atsushi Sato; Mirko Diksic; Tomifusa Kuboki


Japanese Journal of Psychology | 2002

[Development of the behavioral inhibition system/behavioral approach system scales].

Asako Yasuda; Atsushi Sato


Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology | 2000

Is unrealistic optimism really adaptive?: A negative aspect of repressors' optimism.

Asako Yasuda; Atsushi Sato


The Japanese Journal of Personality | 2001

3要因モデルに基づく, 抑うつならびに不安症状の分類 : 多次元抑うつ不安症状尺度の作成

Atsushi Sato; Asako Yasuda; Chiine Kodama

Collaboration


Dive into the Asako Yasuda's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Atsushi Sato

American Physical Therapy Association

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Etsuko Imabayashi

Saitama Medical University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hiroshi Matsuda

Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Yuji Sakano

Health Sciences University of Hokkaido

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge