Bárbara Torres
University of the Basque Country
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Bárbara Torres.
European Journal of Psychological Assessment | 2005
Willem A. Arrindell; Alma Akkerman; Nuri Bagés; Lya Feldman; Vicente E. Caballo; Tian P. S. Oei; Bárbara Torres; Gloria Canalda; Josefina Castro; Im Montgomery; Maryanne Davis; Manuel G. Calvo; Justin Kenardy; David L. Palenzuela; Jeffrey C. Richards; C.C. Leong; M. A. Simón; Flor Zaldívar
The short(s)-EMBU (Swedish acronym for Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostran [My memories of upbringing]) consists of 23 items, is based on the early 81-item EMBU, and was developed out of the necessity of having a brief measure of perceived parental rearing practices when the clinical and/or research context does not adequately permit application of time-consuming test batteries. The s-EMBU comprises three subscales: Rejection., Emotional Warmth, and (Over)Protection. The factorial and/or construct validity and reliability of the s-EMBU were examined in samples comprising a total of 1950 students from Australia, Spain, and Venezuela. The data were presented for the three national groups separately. Findings confirmed the cross-national validity of the factorial structure underlying the s-EMBU. Rejection by fathers and mothers was consistently associated with high trait-neuroticism and low self-esteem in recipients of both sexes in each nation, as was high parental emotional warmth with high femininity (humility). The findings on factorial validity are in keeping with previous ones obtained in East Germany, Greece, Guatemala, Hungary, Italy, and Sweden. The s-EMBU is again recommended for use in several different countries as. a reliable, functional equivalent to the original 81-item EMBU.
Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2003
Willem A. Arrindell; Martin Eisemann; Jörg Richter; Tian P. S. Oei; Vicente E. Caballo; Jan van der Ende; Ezio Sanavio; Nuri Bagés; Lya Feldman; Bárbara Torres; Claudio Sica; Saburo Iwawaki; Robert J. Edelmann; W. Ray Crozier; Adrian Furnham; Barbara L. Hudson
The Fear Survey Schedule-III (FSS-III) was administered to a total of 5491 students in Australia, East Germany, Great Britain, Greece, Guatemala, Hungary, Italy, Japan, Spain, Sweden, and Venezuela, and submitted to the multiple group method of confirmatory analysis (MGM) in order to determine the cross-national dimensional constancy of the five-factor model of self-assessed fears originally established in Dutch, British, and Canadian samples. The model comprises fears of bodily injury-illness-death, agoraphobic fears, social fears, fears of sexual and aggressive scenes, and harmless animals fears. Close correspondence between the factors was demonstrated across national samples. In each country, the corresponding scales were internally consistent, were intercorrelated at magnitudes comparable to those yielded in the original samples, and yielded (in 93% of the total number of 55 comparisons) sex differences in line with the usual finding (higher scores for females). In each country, the relatively largest sex differences were obtained on harmless animals fears. The organization of self-assessed fears is sufficiently similar across nations to warrant the use of the same weight matrix (scoring key) for the FSS-III in the different countries and to make cross-national comparisons feasible. This opens the way to further studies that attempt to predict (on an a priori basis) cross-national variations in fear levels with dimensions of national cultures.
Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2003
Willem A. Arrindell; Martin Eisemann; Jörg Richter; Tian P. S. Oei; Vincente E. Caballo; Jan van der Ende; Ezio Sanavio; Nuri Bagés; Lya Feldman; Bárbara Torres; Claudio Sica; Saburo Iwawaki; Chryse Hatzichristou
Hofstedes dimension of national culture termed Masculinity-Femininity [. Cultures and organizations: software of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill] is proposed to be of relevance for understanding national-level differences in self-assessed agoraphobic fears. This prediction is based on the classical work of Fodor [. In: V. Franks & V. Burtle (Eds.), Women in therapy: new psychotherapies for a changing society. New York: Brunner/Mazel]. A unique data set comprising 11 countries (total N=5491 students) provided the opportunity of scrutinizing this issue. It was hypothesized and found that national Masculinity (the degree to which cultures delineate sex roles, with masculine or tough societies making clearer differentiations between the sexes than feminine or modest societies do) would correlate positively with national agoraphobic fear levels (as assessed with the Fear Survey Schedule-III). Following the correction for sex and age differences across national samples, a significant and large effect-sized national-level (ecological) r=+0.67 (P=0.01) was found. A highly feminine society such as Sweden had the lowest, whereas the champion among the masculine societies, Japan, had the highest national Agoraphobic fear score.
Infancia Y Aprendizaje | 1995
Joaquín de Paúl; María Ignacia Arruabarrena; Bárbara Torres; Ramón Muñoz
ResumenSe presenta un estudio epidemiologico del maltrato infantil en la provincia de Gipuzkoa. Se realizo una cuantificacion y categorizacion de los expedientes de proteccion infantil y un estudio de los casos que determinados profesionales (pediatras, maestros, escuelas infantiles, trabajadores sociales de municipios) identificaron como de maltrato infantil. Se analizaron 189 expedientes abiertos en los que predomina el alto porcentaje de casos de abandono fisico (50%) y la baja tasa de casos de abuso sexual detectados (1%). Los profesionales encuestados identificaron un total de 884 casos de maltrato infantil, en los que destaca el mismo porcentaje de casos de abandono fisico y el aumento del porcentaje de casos de maltrato y abandono emocional. Realizando las extrapolaciones pertinentes y controlando el posible solapamiento de casos detectados por diferentes fuentes profesionales, se puede considerar que la tasa de prevalencia del maltrato infantil en la provincia de Gipuzkoa en 1989 era del 15 por mi...
Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2003
Willem A. Arrindell; Martin Eisemann; Jörg Richter; T.Ps Oei; Vincente E. Caballo; J. van der Ende; Ezio Sanavio; Nuri Bagés; Lya Feldman; Bárbara Torres; Claudio Sica; Saburo Iwawaki; Robert J. Edelmann; Wr Crozier; Adrian Furnham; Barbara L. Hudson
The Fear Survey Schedule-III (FSS-III) was administered to a total of 5491 students in Australia, East Germany, Great Britain, Greece, Guatemala, Hungary, Italy, Japan, Spain, Sweden, and Venezuela, and submitted to the multiple group method of confirmatory analysis (MGM) in order to determine the cross-national dimensional constancy of the five-factor model of self-assessed fears originally established in Dutch, British, and Canadian samples. The model comprises fears of bodily injury-illness-death, agoraphobic fears, social fears, fears of sexual and aggressive scenes, and harmless animals fears. Close correspondence between the factors was demonstrated across national samples. In each country, the corresponding scales were internally consistent, were intercorrelated at magnitudes comparable to those yielded in the original samples, and yielded (in 93% of the total number of 55 comparisons) sex differences in line with the usual finding (higher scores for females). In each country, the relatively largest sex differences were obtained on harmless animals fears. The organization of self-assessed fears is sufficiently similar across nations to warrant the use of the same weight matrix (scoring key) for the FSS-III in the different countries and to make cross-national comparisons feasible. This opens the way to further studies that attempt to predict (on an a priori basis) cross-national variations in fear levels with dimensions of national cultures.
Infancia Y Aprendizaje | 2012
Alexander Muela; Bárbara Torres; Nekane Balluerka
Resumen Los objetivos del presente trabajo consistieron en examinar si las experiencias de maltrato en la infancia influyen en el apego y en la psicopatología, así como en explorar posibles diferencias entre los jóvenes víctimas de maltrato con distintos tipos de intervenciones protectoras. En el estudio participaron 69 jóvenes víctimas de maltrato infantil que se hallaban en situación de acogimiento residencial, 75 jóvenes víctimas de maltrato infantil que se encontraban en situación de riesgo de desamparo y 174 jóvenes que no habían sufrido maltrato. Los resultados indican que sufrir maltrato antes de los 13 años representa un factor de riesgo para mostrar un apego inseguro y psicopatología en la adolescencia. Además, se encontraron algunas diferencias en psicopatología entre los dos grupos de jóvenes víctimas de maltrato infantil, las cuales podrían ser consecuencia de las distintas estrategias defensivas asociadas al estilo de apego constatadas entre ambos grupos.
Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2003
Willem A. Arrindell; Martin Eisemann; Jörg Richter; Tps Oei; Vincente E. Caballo; J. van der Ende; Ezio Sanavio; Nuri Bagés; Lya Feldman; Bárbara Torres; Claudio Sica; Saburo Iwawaki; Robert J. Edelmann; Wr Crozier; Adrian Furnham; Barbara L. Hudson
The Fear Survey Schedule-III (FSS-III) was administered to a total of 5491 students in Australia, East Germany, Great Britain, Greece, Guatemala, Hungary, Italy, Japan, Spain, Sweden, and Venezuela, and submitted to the multiple group method of confirmatory analysis (MGM) in order to determine the cross-national dimensional constancy of the five-factor model of self-assessed fears originally established in Dutch, British, and Canadian samples. The model comprises fears of bodily injury-illness-death, agoraphobic fears, social fears, fears of sexual and aggressive scenes, and harmless animals fears. Close correspondence between the factors was demonstrated across national samples. In each country, the corresponding scales were internally consistent, were intercorrelated at magnitudes comparable to those yielded in the original samples, and yielded (in 93% of the total number of 55 comparisons) sex differences in line with the usual finding (higher scores for females). In each country, the relatively largest sex differences were obtained on harmless animals fears. The organization of self-assessed fears is sufficiently similar across nations to warrant the use of the same weight matrix (scoring key) for the FSS-III in the different countries and to make cross-national comparisons feasible. This opens the way to further studies that attempt to predict (on an a priori basis) cross-national variations in fear levels with dimensions of national cultures.
Personality and Individual Differences | 2004
Willem A. Arrindell; Martin Eisemann; Tian P. S. Oei; Vincente E. Caballo; Ezio Sanavio; Claudio Sica; Nuri Bagés; Lya Feldman; Bárbara Torres; Saburo Iwawaki; Chryse Hatzichristou; Josefina Castro; Gloria Canalda; Adrian Furnham; J. van der Ende
Anales De Psicologia | 2013
Alexander Muela; Nekane Balluerka; Bárbara Torres
Spanish Journal of Psychology | 2011
Bárbara Torres; Itziar Alonso-Arbiol; María José Cantero; Amina Abubakar