Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Benedict C. Jones is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Benedict C. Jones.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2002

Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape

Anthony C. Little; Benedict C. Jones; Ian S. Penton-Voak; D. M. Burt; David I. Perrett

Secondary sexual characteristics may indicate quality of the immune system and therefore a preference for masculinity may confer genetic benefits to offspring; however, high masculinity may be associated with costs of decreased paternal investment. The current study examined womens preferences for masculinity in male faces by using computer graphics to allow transformation between feminine and masculine versions of individual male faces. We found that preferences for masculinity are increased when women either have a partner or are considering a short–term relationship. Such preferences are potentially adaptive, serving to: (i) maximize parental investment and cooperation in long–term relationships by biasing choices towards feminine faced males, and (ii) maximize possible good–gene benefits of short–term or extra–pair partners by biasing choices towards masculine faced males. We also found that individuals using oral contraception do not show the above effects, indicating that such hormonal intervention potentially disrupts womens choices for evolutionarily relevant benefits from males.


Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B | 2011

Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research

Anthony C. Little; Benedict C. Jones; Lisa M. DeBruine

Face preferences affect a diverse range of critical social outcomes, from mate choices and decisions about platonic relationships to hiring decisions and decisions about social exchange. Firstly, we review the facial characteristics that influence attractiveness judgements of faces (e.g. symmetry, sexually dimorphic shape cues, averageness, skin colour/texture and cues to personality) and then review several important sources of individual differences in face preferences (e.g. hormone levels and fertility, own attractiveness and personality, visual experience, familiarity and imprinting, social learning). The research relating to these issues highlights flexible, sophisticated systems that support and promote adaptive responses to faces that appear to function to maximize the benefits of both our mate choices and more general decisions about other types of social partners.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2001

Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness

Ian S. Penton-Voak; Benedict C. Jones; Anthony C. Little; S. Baker; B. Tiddeman; D. M. Burt; David I. Perrett

Facial symmetry has been proposed as a marker of developmental stability that may be important in human mate choice. Several studies have demonstrated positive relationships between facial symmetry and attractiveness. It was recently proposed that symmetry is not a primary cue to facial attractiveness, as symmetrical faces remain attractive even when presented as half faces (with no cues to symmetry). Facial sexual dimorphisms (‘masculinity’) have been suggested as a possible cue that may covary with symmetry in men following data on trait size/symmetry relationships in other species. Here, we use real and computer graphic male faces in order to demonstrate that (i) symmetric faces are more attractive, but not reliably more masculine than less symmetric faces and (ii) that symmetric faces possess characteristics that are attractive independent of symmetry, but that these characteristics remain at present undefined.


Hormones and Behavior | 2006

Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice

David R. Feinberg; Benedict C. Jones; M.J. Law Smith; Fhionna R. Moore; Lisa M. DeBruine; R.E. Cornwell; S. G. Hillier; David I. Perrett

Men with low testosterone (feminine men) invest in relationships and offspring more than men with high testosterone (masculine men). Womens attraction to testosterone dependent traits (e.g. masculine face shape) is enhanced during the late-follicular, fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Attractive, feminine women have stronger preferences for masculine men as possible long-term partners than less attractive, masculine women. We manipulated 2 testosterone related vocal traits (voice pitch and apparent vocal-tract length) in voices to test if women prefer masculinized mens voices to feminized mens voices; masculinity preferences are enhanced at the fertile (late-follicular) menstrual cycle phase; the amount that masculinity preferences shift cyclically relates to average estrone-3-glucuronide concentration (the primary urinary metabolite of estrone, E3G). We found women displayed general masculinity preferences for mens voices; masculinity preferences were greater in the fertile (late-follicular) phase of the cycle than the non-fertile (early-follicular and luteal) phase; and this effect was most pronounced for women with low average E3G concentration. As feminine women (i.e. those with high average E3G levels) are most able to obtain investment even from masculine men, these women may not need to change their mating preference or strategy during the menstrual cycle as much as masculine women.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2006

Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women

M.J. Law Smith; David I. Perrett; Benedict C. Jones; R.E. Cornwell; Fhionna R. Moore; David R. Feinberg; Lynda G. Boothroyd; S. J. Durrani; Michael Stirrat; S. Whiten; R. M. Pitman; S. G. Hillier

Although many accounts of facial attractiveness propose that femininity in womens faces indicates high levels of oestrogen, there is little empirical evidence in support of this assumption. Here, we used assays for urinary metabolites of oestrogen (oestrone-3-glucuronide, E1G) and progesterone (pregnanediol-3-glucuronide, P3G) to investigate the relationship between circulating gonadal hormones and ratings of the femininity, attractiveness and apparent health of womens faces. Positive correlations were observed between late follicular oestrogen and ratings of femininity, attractiveness and health. Positive correlations of luteal progesterone and health and attractiveness ratings were marginally significant. Ratings of facial attributions did not relate to hormone levels for women wearing make-up when photographed. There was no effect of sex of rater on the relationships between oestrogen and ratings of facial appearance. These findings demonstrate that female facial appearance holds detectable cues to reproductive health that are considered attractive by other people.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2004

Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle

S. Craig Roberts; Jan Havlíček; Jaroslav Flegr; Martina Hrušková; Anthony C. Little; Benedict C. Jones; David I. Perrett; Marion Petrie

The lack of obvious visible manifestations of ovulation in human females, compared with the prominent sexual swellings of many primates, has led to the idea that human ovulation is concealed. While human ovulation is clearly not advertised to the same extent as in some other species, we show here that both men and women judge photographs of womens faces that were taken in the fertile window of the menstrual cycle as more attractive than photographs taken during the luteal phase. This indicates the existence of visible cues to ovulation in the human face, and is consistent with similar cyclical changes observed for preferences of female body odour. This heightened allure could be an adaptive mechanism for raising a females relative value in the mating market at the time in the cycle when the probability of conception is at its highest.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2006

Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner's masculinity

Lisa M. DeBruine; Benedict C. Jones; Anthony C. Little; Lynda G. Boothroyd; David I. Perrett; Ian S. Penton-Voak; Philip A. Cooper; Lars Penke; David R. Feinberg; Bernard Tiddeman

Studies of womens preferences for male faces have variously reported preferences for masculine faces, preferences for feminine faces and no effect of masculinity–femininity on male facial attractiveness. It has been suggested that these apparently inconsistent findings are, at least partly, due to differences in the methods used to manipulate the masculinity of face images or individual differences in attraction to facial cues associated with youth. Here, however, we show that womens preferences for masculinity manipulated in male faces using techniques similar to the three most widely used methods are positively inter-related. We also show that womens preferences for masculine male faces are positively related to ratings of the masculinity of their actual partner and their ideal partner. Correlations with partner masculinity were independent of real and ideal partner age, which were not associated with facial masculinity preference. Collectively, these findings suggest that variability among studies in their findings for womens masculinity preferences reflects individual differences in attraction to masculinity rather than differences in the methods used to manufacture stimuli, and are important for the interpretation of previous and future studies of facial masculinity.


Hormones and Behavior | 2005

Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high.

Benedict C. Jones; Anthony C. Little; Lynda G. Boothroyd; Lisa M. DeBruine; David R. Feinberg; M.J. Law Smith; R.E. Cornwell; Fhionna R. Moore; David I. Perrett

Previous studies of changes in womens behavior during the menstrual cycle have offered insight into the motivations underpinning womens preferences for social cues associated with possible direct benefits (e.g., investment, low risk of infection) and indirect benefits (e.g., offspring viability). Here we sought to extend this work by testing for systematic variation in womens preferences for male and female faces and in their attitudes to their romantic relationship during the menstrual cycle. In Study 1, we found partnered womens reported commitment to their romantic relationship and preferences for femininity in male and female faces were strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone levels are increased (and fertility is low). Happiness in relationships did not change across the cycle. In Study 2, we found that the effect of cycle phase on womens preference for feminine faces was independent of increased attraction to apparent health in faces during the luteal phase. Collectively, these findings are further evidence that womens preferences for social cues associated with possible direct benefits and commitment to relationships are strongest during conditions characterized by raised progesterone level, while attraction to men displaying cues associated with possible indirect benefits is strongest when women are most fertile.


Hormones and Behavior | 2007

Raised salivary testosterone in women is associated with increased attraction to masculine faces.

Lisa L. M. Welling; Benedict C. Jones; Lisa M. DeBruine; Claire A. Conway; M.J. Law Smith; Anthony C. Little; David R. Feinberg; M A Sharp; Emad A S Al-Dujaili

Womens preferences for masculinity in mens faces, voices and behavioral displays change during the menstrual cycle and are strongest around ovulation. While previous findings suggest that change in progesterone level is an important hormonal mechanism for such variation, it is likely that changes in the levels of other hormones will also contribute to cyclic variation in masculinity preferences. Here we compared womens preferences for masculine faces at two points in the menstrual cycle where women differed in salivary testosterone, but not in salivary progesterone or estrogen. Preferences for masculinity were strongest when womens testosterone levels were relatively high. Our findings complement those from previous studies that show systematic variation in masculinity preferences during the menstrual cycle and suggest that change in testosterone level may play an important role in cyclic shifts in womens preferences for masculine traits.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2010

The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for masculinized male faces

Lisa M. DeBruine; Benedict C. Jones; John R. Crawford; Lisa L. M. Welling; Anthony C. Little

Recent formulations of sexual selection theory emphasize how mate choice can be affected by environmental factors, such as predation risk and resource quality. Women vary greatly in the extent to which they prefer male masculinity and this variation is hypothesized to reflect differences in how women resolve the trade-off between the costs (e.g. low investment) and benefits (e.g. healthy offspring) associated with choosing a masculine partner. A strong prediction of this trade-off theory is that womens masculinity preferences will be stronger in cultures where poor health is particularly harmful to survival. We investigated the relationship between womens preferences for male facial masculinity and a health index derived from World Health Organization statistics for mortality rates, life expectancies and the impact of communicable disease. Across 30 countries, masculinity preference increased as health decreased. This relationship was independent of cross-cultural differences in wealth or womens mating strategies. These findings show non-arbitrary cross-cultural differences in facial attractiveness judgements and demonstrate the use of trade-off theory for investigating cross-cultural variation in womens mate preferences.

Collaboration


Dive into the Benedict C. Jones's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Amanda C. Hahn

Humboldt State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge