Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Benjawan Kasisopa is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Benjawan Kasisopa.


Applied Psycholinguistics | 2015

Universality and language-specific experience in the perception of lexical tone and pitch

Denis Burnham; Benjawan Kasisopa; Amanda Reid; Sudaporn Luksaneeyanawin; Francisco Lacerda; Virginie Attina; Nan Xu Rattanasone; Iris-Corinna Schwarz; Diane Webster

Two experiments focus on Thai tone perception by native speakers of tone languages (Thai, Cantonese, and Mandarin), a pitch–accent (Swedish), and a nontonal (English) language. In Experiment 1, there was better auditory-only and auditory–visual discrimination by tone and pitch–accent language speakers than by nontone language speakers. Conversely and counterintuitively, there was better visual-only discrimination by nontone language speakers than tone and pitch–accent language speakers. Nevertheless, visual augmentation of auditory tone perception in noise was evident for all five language groups. In Experiment 2, involving discrimination in three fundamental frequency equivalent auditory contexts, tone and pitch–accent language participants showed equivalent discrimination for normal Thai speech, filtered speech, and violin sounds. In contrast, nontone language listeners had significantly better discrimination for violin sounds than filtered speech and in turn speech. Together the results show that tone perception is determined by both auditory and visual information, by acoustic and linguistic contexts, and by universal and experiential factors.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2011

Are tones phones

Denis Burnham; Jeesun Kim; Chris Davis; Valter Ciocca; Colin Schoknecht; Benjawan Kasisopa; Sudaporn Luksaneeyanawin

The psycholinguistic status of lexical tones and phones is indexed via phonological and tonological awareness (PA and TA, respectively) using Thai speech. In Experiment 1 (Thai participants, alphabetic script and orthographically explicit phones/tones), PA was better than TA in children and primary school-educated adults, and TA improved to PA levels only in tertiary-educated adults. In Experiment 2 (Cantonese participants, logographic script and no orthographically explicit phones/tones), children and primary-educated adults had better PA than TA, and PA and TA were equivalent in tertiary-educated adults, but were nevertheless still below the level of their Thai counterparts. Experiment 3 (English-language participants, alphabetic script and nontonal) showed better PA than TA. Regression analyses showed that both TA and PA are predicted by reading ability for Thai children but by general nonorthographic age-related variables for Cantonese children, whereas for English children reading ability predicts PA but not TA. The results show a phone>tone perceptual advantage over both age and languages that is affected by availability of orthographically relevant information and metalinguistic maturity. More generally, both the perception and the psycholinguistic representation of phones and tones differ.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2015

Perceptual assimilation of lexical tone: The roles of language experience and visual information

Amanda Reid; Denis Burnham; Benjawan Kasisopa; Ronan G. Reilly; Virginie Attina; Nan Xu Rattanasone; Catherine T. Best

Using Best’s (1995) perceptual assimilation model (PAM), we investigated auditory–visual (AV), auditory-only (AO), and visual-only (VO) perception of Thai tones. Mandarin and Cantonese (tone-language) speakers were asked to categorize Thai tones according to their own native tone categories, and Australian English (non-tone-language) speakers to categorize Thai tones into their native intonation categories—for instance, question or statement. As comparisons, Thai participants completed a straightforward identification task, and another Australian English group identified the Thai tones using simple symbols. All of the groups also completed an AX discrimination task. Both the Mandarin and Cantonese groups categorized AO and AV Thai falling tones as their native level tones, and Thai rising tones as their native rising tones, although the Mandarin participants found it easier to categorize Thai level tones than did the Cantonese participants. VO information led to very poor categorization for all groups, and AO and AV information also led to very poor categorizations for the English intonation categorization group. PAM’s predictions regarding tone discriminability based on these category assimilation patterns were borne out for the Mandarin group’s AO and AV discriminations, providing support for the applicability of the PAM to lexical tones. For the Cantonese group, however, PAM was unable to account for one specific discrimination pattern—namely, their relatively good performance on the Thai high–rising contrast in the auditory conditions—and no predictions could be derived for the English groups. A full account of tone assimilation will likely need to incorporate considerations of phonetic, and even acoustic, similarity and overlap between nonnative and native tone categories.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2010

Speech articulator movements recorded from facing talkers using two electromagnetic articulometer systems simultaneously

Mark Tiede; Rikke L. Bundgaard-Nielsen; Christian Kroos; Guillaume Gibert; Virginie Attina; Benjawan Kasisopa; Eric Vatikiotis-Bateson; Catherine T. Best

Two 3‐D electromagnetic articulometer systems, the Carstens AG500 and Northern Digital WAVE, have been used simultaneously without mutual interference to record the speech articulator movements of two talkers facing one another 2 m apart. A series of benchmark tests evaluating the stability of fixed distances between sensors attached to a rotating rigid body was first conducted to determine whether the two systems could operate independently, with results showing no significant effect of dual operation on either system. In the experiment proper, two native speakers of American English participated as subjects. Sensors were glued to three points on the tongue, the upper and lower incisors, lips, and left and right mastoid processes for each subject. Independent audio tracks were recorded using separate directional microphones, which were used to align the kinematic data from both subjects during post‐processing. Data collected were of two types: extended spontaneous conversation and repeated incongruent word sequences (e.g., talker one produced “cop top...;” talker two “top cop...”). Both talkers show strong positive correlations between speech rate (in syllables/s) and head movement. The word sequences also show error and rate effects related to mutual entrainment. [Work supported by ARC Human Communication Science Network (RN0460284), MARCS Auditory Laboratories, NIH.]


Behavioral and Brain Sciences | 2012

What and where is the word

Catherine McBride-Chang; Hsuan-Chih Chen; Benjawan Kasisopa; Denis Burnham; Ronan G. Reilly; Paavo H. T. Leppänen

Examples from Chinese, Thai, and Finnish illustrate why researchers cannot always be confident about the precise nature of the word unit. Understanding ambiguities regarding where a word begins and ends, and how to model word recognition when many derivations of a word are possible, is essential for universal theories of reading applied to both developing and expert readers.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2018

Training Children to Perceive Non-native Lexical Tones: Tone Language Background, Bilingualism, and Auditory-Visual Information

Benjawan Kasisopa; Lamya El-Khoury Antonios; Allard Jongman; Joan A. Sereno; Denis Burnham

This study investigates the role of language background and bilingual status in the perception of foreign lexical tones. Eight groups of participants, consisting of children of 6 and 8 years from one of four language background (tone or non-tone) × bilingual status (monolingual or bilingual)—Thai monolingual, English monolingual, English-Thai bilingual, and English-Arabic bilingual were trained to perceive the four Mandarin lexical tones. Half the children in each of these eight groups were given auditory-only (AO) training and half auditory-visual (AV) training. In each group Mandarin tone identification was tested before and after (pre- and post-) training with both auditory-only test (ao-test) and auditory-visual test (av test). The effect of training on Mandarin tone identification was minimal for 6-year-olds. On the other hand, 8-year-olds, particularly those with tone language experience showed greater pre- to post-training improvement, and this was best indexed by ao-test trials. Bilingual vs. monolingual background did not facilitate overall improvement due to training, but it did modulate the efficacy of the Training mode: for bilinguals both AO and AV training, and especially AO, resulted in performance gain; but for monolinguals training was most effective with AV stimuli. Again this effect was best indexed by ao-test trials. These results suggest that tone language experience, be it monolingual or bilingual, is a strong predictor of learning unfamiliar tones; that monolinguals learn best from AV training trials and bilinguals from AO training trials; and that there is no metalinguistic advantage due to bilingualism in learning to perceive lexical tones.


Vision Research | 2013

Eye movements while reading an unspaced writing system: the case of Thai.

Benjawan Kasisopa; Ronan G. Reilly; Sudaporn Luksaneeyanawin; Denis Burnham


Proceedings of the International Conference on Audio-Visual Speech Processing (AVSP2011), Aug 31 - Sep 3, 2011, Volterra, Italy | 2011

Auditory-visual discrimination and identification of lexical tone within and across tone languages

Denis Burnham; Virginie Attina; Benjawan Kasisopa


Vision Research | 2016

Child readers’ eye movements in reading Thai

Benjawan Kasisopa; Ronan G. Reilly; Sudaporn Luksaneeyanawin; Denis Burnham


South and Southeast Asian Psycholinguistics | 2014

How to compare tones

Nan Xu Rattanasone; Virginie Attina; Benjawan Kasisopa; Denis Burnham

Collaboration


Dive into the Benjawan Kasisopa's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Virginie Attina

University of Western Sydney

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Catherine T. Best

University of Western Sydney

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Amanda Reid

University of Western Sydney

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Christian Kroos

University of Western Sydney

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mark Tiede

Massachusetts Institute of Technology

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge