Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Brenda McCowan is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Brenda McCowan.


Animal Behaviour | 2004

Barking in domestic dogs: context specificity and individual identification

Sophia Yin; Brenda McCowan

In this study we sought to determine whether dog barks could be divided into subtypes based on context. We recorded barking from 10 adult dogs, Canis familiaris, of six breeds in three different test situations: (1) a disturbance situation in which a stranger rang the doorbell, (2) an isolation situation in which the dog was locked outside or in a room isolated from its owner and (3) a play situation in which either two dogs or a human and a dog played together. We analysed spectrograms of 4672 barks using macros that took 60 sequential frequency measurements and 60 sequential amplitude measurements along the length of the call. Statistical analyses revealed that barks are graded vocalizations that range from harsh, low-frequency, unmodulated calls to harmonically rich, higher-frequency, modulated calls. The harsh, low-frequency, unmodulated barks were more commonly given in the disturbance situation, and the more tonal, higherpitch, modulated barks were more commonly given in the isolation and play situations. Disturbance barks were also longer in duration with more rapid repetition than the barks given in other contexts. Discriminant analysis revealed that dog barks can be divided into different subtypes based on context even within individual dogs, and that dogs can be identified by their bark spectrograms despite the context of the bark.


Biological Reviews | 2016

Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus

Arik Kershenbaum; Daniel T. Blumstein; Marie A. Roch; Çağlar Akçay; Gregory A. Backus; Mark A. Bee; Kirsten Bohn; Yan Cao; Gerald G. Carter; Cristiane Cäsar; Michael H. Coen; Stacy L. DeRuiter; Laurance R. Doyle; Shimon Edelman; Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho; Todd M. Freeberg; Ellen C. Garland; Morgan L. Gustison; Heidi E. Harley; Chloé Huetz; Melissa Hughes; Julia Hyland Bruno; Amiyaal Ilany; Dezhe Z. Jin; Michael T. Johnson; Chenghui Ju; Jeremy Karnowski; Bernard Lohr; Marta B. Manser; Brenda McCowan

Animal acoustic communication often takes the form of complex sequences, made up of multiple distinct acoustic units. Apart from the well‐known example of birdsong, other animals such as insects, amphibians, and mammals (including bats, rodents, primates, and cetaceans) also generate complex acoustic sequences. Occasionally, such as with birdsong, the adaptive role of these sequences seems clear (e.g. mate attraction and territorial defence). More often however, researchers have only begun to characterise – let alone understand – the significance and meaning of acoustic sequences. Hypotheses abound, but there is little agreement as to how sequences should be defined and analysed. Our review aims to outline suitable methods for testing these hypotheses, and to describe the major limitations to our current and near‐future knowledge on questions of acoustic sequences. This review and prospectus is the result of a collaborative effort between 43 scientists from the fields of animal behaviour, ecology and evolution, signal processing, machine learning, quantitative linguistics, and information theory, who gathered for a 2013 workshop entitled, ‘Analysing vocal sequences in animals’. Our goal is to present not just a review of the state of the art, but to propose a methodological framework that summarises what we suggest are the best practices for research in this field, across taxa and across disciplines. We also provide a tutorial‐style introduction to some of the most promising algorithmic approaches for analysing sequences. We divide our review into three sections: identifying the distinct units of an acoustic sequence, describing the different ways that information can be contained within a sequence, and analysing the structure of that sequence. Each of these sections is further subdivided to address the key questions and approaches in that area. We propose a uniform, systematic, and comprehensive approach to studying sequences, with the goal of clarifying research terms used in different fields, and facilitating collaboration and comparative studies. Allowing greater interdisciplinary collaboration will facilitate the investigation of many important questions in the evolution of communication and sociality.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2002

Individual acoustic variation in Belding's ground squirrel alarm chirps in the High Sierra Nevada (L)

Brenda McCowan; Stacie L. Hooper

The acoustic structure of calls within call types can vary as function of individual identity, sex, and social group membership and is important in kin and social group recognition. Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi) produce alarm chirps that function in predator avoidance but little is known about the acoustic variability of these alarm chirps. The purpose of this preliminary study was to analyze the acoustic structure of alarm chirps with respect to individual differences (e.g., signature information) from eight Belding’s ground squirrels from four different lakes in the High Sierra Nevada. Results demonstrate that alarm chirps are individually distinctive, and that acoustic similarity among individuals may correspond to genetic similarity and thus dispersal patterns in this species. These data suggest, on a preliminary basis, that the acoustic structure of calls might be used as a bioacoustic tool for tracking individuals, dispersal, and other population dynamics in Belding’s ground squirrels, and perhaps other vocal species.


American Journal of Primatology | 2013

Risk factors for stereotypic behavior and self-biting in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): Animal's history, current environment, and personality

Daniel H. Gottlieb; John P. Capitanio; Brenda McCowan

Captive rhesus macaques sometimes exhibit undesirable abnormal behaviors, such as motor stereotypic behavior (MSB) and self‐abuse. Many risk factors for these behaviors have been identified but the list is far from comprehensive, and large individual differences in rate of behavior expression remain. The goal of the current study was to determine which experiences predict expression of MSB and self‐biting, and if individual differences in personality can account for additional variation in MSB expression. A risk factor analysis was performed utilizing data from over 4,000 rhesus monkeys at the California National Primate Research Center. Data were analyzed using model selection, with the best fitting models evaluated using Akaike Information Criterion. Results confirmed previous research that males exhibit more MSB and self‐biting than females, MSB decreases with age, and indoor reared animals exhibit more MSB and self‐biting than outdoor reared animals. Additionally, results indicated that animals exhibited less MSB and self‐biting for each year spent outdoors; frequency of room moves and number of projects positively predicted MSB; pair separations positively predicted MSB and self‐biting; pair housed animals expressed less MSB than single housed and grate paired animals; and that animals expressed more MSB and self‐biting when in bottom rack cages, or cages near the room entrance. Based on these results we recommend limiting exposure to these risk factors when possible. Our results also demonstrated a relationship between personality and MSB expression, with animals low on gentle temperament, active in response to a human intruder, and high on novel object contact expressing more MSB. From these results we propose that an animals MSB is related to its predisposition for an active personality, with active animals expressing higher rates of MSB. Am. J. Primatol. 75:995–1008, 2013.


Animal Behaviour | 2008

Amplitude of bison bellows reflects male quality, physical condition and motivation

Megan T. Wyman; Michael S. Mooring; Brenda McCowan; M. Cecilia T. Penedo; Lynette A. Hart

Sound amplitude (measured as sound pressure level) is an acoustical parameter that has received little attention within communication research, especially in mammals. Although difficult to measure in the field, amplitude is a potentially important parameter of sexually selected signals. In North American plains bison, Bison bison, ‘bellows’ are low, guttural vocalizations made by bulls during the breeding season in the context of male–male contests. It has been hypothesized that bison use bellow amplitude to assess males during male–male competition or female mate choice. In this study, we tested the hypothesis that amplitude is significantly related to measures of bull competitive ability (quality, condition and motivation), and thus, could function as a sexually selected signal. During peak rut over 2 years, courtship and threat behaviours were recorded daily. During observation sessions, bellow amplitude was measured as peak sound pressure levels using a sound level meter. Subsequent genetic parentage analysis determined offspring sired by males. Based on aspects of signalling and game theory, we predicted positive associations between amplitude and mating and reproductive success, dominance, physical condition, motivation to retain females, age and weight. Our results supported a positive association between amplitude and both physical condition and motivation. Conversely, the results showed a negative association between amplitude and quality, as measured by mating and reproductive success. Supporting evidence and alternative hypotheses for these results are explored. Our findings provide support for the notion that bellow amplitude could be used as a sexually selected signal to assess rival males during male–male competition.


Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science | 2009

Risk Factors and Remediation of Self-Injurious and Self-Abuse Behavior in Rhesus Macaques

Ina Rommeck; Kristen Anderson; Allison Heagerty; Ashley N. Cameron; Brenda McCowan

Considered signs of decreased welfare—abnormal behaviors such as self-injury and self-abuse among nonhuman primates housed in the laboratory—may put into question the validity and reliability of scientific research using these animals as models. Providing environmental enrichment decreases the incidence of some undesirable behaviors but is often unsuccessful at ameliorating the most severe types of abnormal behaviors. To prevent such behaviors from developing, it is important to identify risk factors that provide insight into the causes of certain abnormal behaviors. This study confirmed previous research identifying nursery rearing, single housing, and time spent in single housing as important risk factors. Results also indicate that the number of cage relocations affects the development of these behaviors. In addition, this study presents new data on comorbidity of several abnormal behaviors and discusses possible reasons for these patterns.


Biological Reviews | 2008

A claim in search of evidence: reply to Manger's thermogenesis hypothesis of cetacean brain structure

Lori Marino; Camilla Butti; Richard C. Connor; R. Ewan Fordyce; Louis M. Herman; Patrick R. Hof; Louis Lefebvre; David Lusseau; Brenda McCowan; Esther A. Nimchinsky; Adam A. Pack; Joy S. Reidenberg; Diana Reiss; Luke Rendell; Mark D. Uhen; Estelle Van der Gucht; Hal Whitehead

In a recent publication in Biological Reviews, Manger (2006) made the controversial claim that the large brains of cetaceans evolved to generate heat during oceanic cooling in the Oligocene epoch and not, as is the currently accepted view, as a basis for an increase in cognitive or information‐processing capabilities in response to ecological or social pressures. Manger further argued that dolphins and other cetaceans are considerably less intelligent than generally thought. In this review we challenge Manger’s arguments and provide abundant evidence that modern cetacean brains are large in order to support complex cognitive abilities driven by social and ecological forces.


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2012

How can social network analysis contribute to social behavior research in applied ethology

Maja M. Makagon; Brenda McCowan; Joy A. Mench

Social network analysis is increasingly used by behavioral ecologists and primatologists to describe the patterns and quality of interactions among individuals. We provide an overview of this methodology, with examples illustrating how it can be used to study social behavior in applied contexts. Like most kinds of social interaction analyses, social network analysis provides information about direct relationships (e.g. dominant-subordinate relationships). However, it also generates a more global model of social organization that determines how individual patterns of social interaction relate to individual and group characteristics. A particular strength of this approach is that it provides standardized mathematical methods for calculating metrics of sociality across levels of social organization, from the population and group levels to the individual level. At the group level these metrics can be used to track changes in social network structures over time, evaluate the effect of the environment on social network structure, or compare social structures across groups, populations or species. At the individual level, the metrics allow quantification of the heterogeneity of social experience within groups and identification of individuals who may play especially important roles in maintaining social stability or information flow throughout the network.


Journal of Comparative Psychology | 2000

Bubble Ring Play of Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Implications for Cognition

Brenda McCowan; Lori Marino; Erik Vance; Leah Walke; Diana Reiss

Research on the cognitive capacities of dolphins and other cetaceans (whales and porpoises) has importance for the study of comparative cognition, particularly with other large-brained social mammals, such as primates. One of the areas in which cetaceans can be compared with primates is that of object manipulation and physical causality, for which there is an abundant body of literature in primates. The authors supplemented qualitative observations with statistical methods to examine playful bouts of underwater bubble ring production and manipulation in 4 juvenile male captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). The results are consistent with the hypothesis that dolphins monitor the quality of their bubble rings and anticipate their actions during bubble ring play.


PLOS ONE | 2011

Network Stability Is a Balancing Act of Personality, Power, and Conflict Dynamics in Rhesus Macaque Societies

Brenda McCowan; Brianne A. Beisner; John P. Capitanio; Megan E. Jackson; Ashley N. Cameron; Shannon K. Seil; Edward R. Atwill; Hsieh Fushing

Stability in biological systems requires evolved mechanisms that promote robustness. Cohesive primate social groups represent one example of a stable biological system, which persist in spite of frequent conflict. Multiple sources of stability likely exist for any biological system and such robustness, or lack thereof, should be reflected and thus detectable in the groups network structure, and likely at multiple levels. Here we show how network structure and group stability are linked to the fundamental characteristics of the individual agents in groups and to the environmental and social contexts in which these individuals interact. Both internal factors (e.g., personality, sex) and external factors (e.g., rank dynamics, sex ratio) were considered from the level of the individual to that of the group to examine the effects of network structure on group stability in a nonhuman primate species. The results yielded three main findings. First, successful third-party intervention behavior is a mechanism of group stability in rhesus macaques in that successful interventions resulted in less wounding in social groups. Second, personality is the primary factor that determines which individuals perform the role of key intervener, via its effect on social power and dominance discrepancy. Finally, individuals with high social power are not only key interveners but also key players in grooming networks and receive reconciliations from a higher diversity of individuals. The results from this study provide sound evidence that individual and group characteristics such as personality and sex ratio influence network structures such as patterns of reconciliation, grooming and conflict intervention that are indicators of network robustness and consequent health and well-being in rhesus macaque societies. Utilizing this network approach has provided greater insight into how behavioral and social processes influence social stability in nonhuman primate groups.

Collaboration


Dive into the Brenda McCowan's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sean F. Hanser

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John P. Capitanio

California National Primate Research Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jessica J. Vandeleest

California National Primate Research Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ashley N. Cameron

California National Primate Research Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Diana Reiss

City University of New York

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge