Brooke M. Smith
Utah State University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Brooke M. Smith.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior | 2017
Brooke M. Smith; Gregory S. Smith; Timothy A. Shahan; Gregory J. Madden; Michael P. Twohig
Despite the success of exposure-based psychotherapies in anxiety treatment, relapse remains problematic. Resurgence, the return of previously eliminated behavior following the elimination of an alternative source of reinforcement, is a promising model of operant relapse. Nonhuman resurgence research has shown that higher rates of alternative reinforcement result in faster, more comprehensive suppression of target behavior, but also in greater resurgence when alternative reinforcement is eliminated. This study investigated rich and lean rates of alternative reinforcement on response suppression and resurgence in typically developing humans. In Phase 1, three groups (Rich, n = 18; Lean, n = 18; Control, n = 10) acquired the target response. In Phase 2, target responding was extinguished and alternative reinforcement delivered on RI 1 s, RI 3 s, and extinction schedules, respectively. Resurgence was assessed during Phase 3 under extinction conditions for all groups. Target responding was suppressed most thoroughly in Rich and partially in Lean. Target responding resurged in the Rich and Lean groups, but not in the Control group. Between groups, resurgence was more pronounced in the Rich group than the Lean and Control groups. Clinical implications of these findings, including care on the part of clinicians when identifying alternative sources of reinforcement, are discussed.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior | 2018
Ann Galizio; Charles C. J. Frye; Jeremy M. Haynes; Jonathan E. Friedel; Brooke M. Smith; Amy L. Odum
The present study examined persistence and relapse of reinforced behavioral variability in pigeons. Pigeons emitted four-response sequences across two keys. Sequences produced food according to a lag schedule, in which a response sequence was followed by food if it differed from a certain number of previous sequences. In Experiment 1, food was delivered for sequences that satisfied a lag schedule in both components of a multiple schedule. When reinforcement was removed for one component (i.e., extinction), levels of behavioral variability decreased for only that component. In Experiment 2, food was delivered for sequences satisfying a lag schedule in one component of a multiple schedule. In the other component, food was delivered at the same rate, but without the lag variability requirement (i.e., yoked). Following extinction, levels of behavioral variability returned to baseline for both components after response-independent food delivery (i.e., reinstatement). In Experiment 3, one group of pigeons responded on a lag variability schedule, and the other group responded on a lag repetition schedule. For both groups, levels of behavioral variability increased when alternative reinforcement was suspended (i.e., resurgence). In each experiment, we observed some evidence for extinction-induced response variability and for variability as an operant dimension of behavior.
Behavior Modification | 2018
Brooke M. Smith; Jennifer L. Villatte; Clarissa W. Ong; Grayson M. Butcher; Michael P. Twohig; Michael E. Levin; Steven C. Hayes
Research has demonstrated that values and acceptance interventions can increase distress tolerance, but the individual contribution of each remains unclear. The current study examined the isolated effect of a values intervention on immersion time in a cold pressor. Participants randomized to Values (n = 18) and Control (n = 14) conditions completed two cold pressor tasks, separated by a 30-min values or control intervention. Immersion time increased 51.06 s for participants in the Values condition and decreased by 10.79 s for those in the Control condition. Increases in self-reported pain and distress predicted decreases in immersion time for Control, but not Values, participants. The best-fitting model accounted for 39% of the variance in immersion time change. Results suggest that a brief isolated values exercise can be used to improve distress tolerance despite increased perceptions of pain and distress, such that values alone may be sufficient to facilitate openness to difficult experiences.
Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders | 2015
Michael P. Twohig; Jonathan S. Abramowitz; Ellen J. Bluett; Laura E. Fabricant; Ryan J. Jacoby; Kate L. Morrison; Lillian Reuman; Brooke M. Smith
Journal of contextual behavioral science | 2017
Eric B. Lee; Brooke M. Smith; Michael P. Twohig; Tera Lensegrav-Benson; Benita Quakenbush-Roberts
Behaviour Research and Therapy | 2018
Michael P. Twohig; Jonathan S. Abramowitz; Brooke M. Smith; Laura E. Fabricant; Ryan J. Jacoby; Kate L. Morrison; Ellen J. Bluett; Lillian Reuman; Shannon M. Blakey; Thomas Ledermann
Archive | 2017
Michael E. Levin; Michael P. Twohig; Brooke M. Smith
Current opinion in psychology | 2015
Michael P. Twohig; Brooke M. Smith
Archive | 2017
Kate L. Morrison; Brooke M. Smith; Eric B. Lee; Michael P. Twohig
Archive | 2017
Brooke M. Smith; Ellen J. Bluett; Eric B. Lee; Michael P. Twohig