Bruno Secco Faquin
Universidade Estadual de Londrina
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Bruno Secco Faquin.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2013
Paulo César Andrean; Bruno Secco Faquin; Juliana Bayeux Dascal; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
The present study analyzed the effect of interlateral transfer direction while learning a touching finger sequence task. Twenty right-handed participants (18-30 years old) were divided into two groups: practice with the right hand (GD) and left hand (GE). Groups were evaluated during pretest, posttest, and retention phases. The task was performing the higher number of finger touches between tomb and the other fingers, during 10 seconds, in a pre-stablished order. The GE showed higher number of finger touchs with the left hand in posttest (Z=2,2; P=0,028) compared to pretest. The GD increased the number of finger touches from pretest to retention for the right hand (Z=2,4; P=0,017) and left hand (Z=2,6; P=0,009), indicating learning and interlateral transfer of learning. The interlateral transfer of learning from the right to the left hand was explained by the principle of trainability.The present study analyzed the effect of interlateral transfer direction while learning a touching finger sequence task. Twenty right-handed participants (18-30 years old) were divided into two groups: practice with the right hand (GD) and left hand (GE). Groups were evaluated during pretest, posttest, and retention phases. The task was performing the higher number of finger touches between tomb and the other fingers, during 10 seconds, in a pre-stablished order. The GE showed higher number of finger touchs with the left hand in posttest (Z=2,2; P=0,028) compared to pretest. The GD increased the number of finger touches from pretest to retention for the right hand (Z=2,4; P=0,017) and left hand (Z=2,6; P=0,009), indicating learning and interlateral transfer of learning. The interlateral transfer of learning from the right to the left hand was explained by the principle of trainability.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2011
Giane Francine Garcia de Moraes; Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Bruno Secco Faquin; Tiago Gorgatti; Inara Marques; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
It was analyzed the athletic performance in basketball players using wheelchairs prescribed (specific) and not prescribed (non-specific) according to the particularity of each player. Six wheelchair basketball players performed the 20 meters speed test (with and without dribbling a ball), and the agility test without dribbling a ball. Scales of effort and comfort were used after each test. It was verified better performance (P<0,05) and comfort (P<0,05) in the tests performed with the use of the prescribed wheelchair. Therefore, it was suggested that the use of prescribed wheelchair specially designed for each athlete, for the optimization of their performance and comfort in the basketball wheelchair sport.
Human Movement | 2018
Bruno Secco Faquin; Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Luis Mochizuki; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
Purpose. Walking demands controlling body segments based on sensory information. Experimental manipulation of sensory information provides insight about how it interacts in situations of occlusion or perturbation of such information. this study examined the effect of manipulation of visual and vestibular information on spatial perception during gait. Methods. thirty-two participants aged between 19-34 years old walked straight ahead for 7 meters to reach the center point of the pathway. the authors measured the number of steps, movement time, and absolute error (deviation from the center of pathway end). they analyzed six experimental conditions: without spin with vision, without spin with visual occlusion, without spin with visual perturbation, with spin and vision, with spin and visual occlusion, and with spin and visual perturbation. Friedman’s ANOVA test was used to compare the experimental conditions with a significance of 5% (p < 0.05). Results. Occlusion and perturbation of visual information, and the perturbation of vestibular information, both affected the spatial orientation. the more sources of information that were occluded and/or perturbed; the greater was the decline of spatial perception. However, participants were able to perform the task of walking in all conditions with the manipulation of sensory information. Conclusions. the authors suggest that there is a dynamic hierarchy in which the sensory sources can contribute in a particular way, due to the constraint imposed on the system.
Revista da Educação Física/UEM | 2013
Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Bruno Secco Faquin; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
The present study compared internal focus (IF) and external focus (EF) of attention and analyzed the constrained action hypothesis. For such, the balance performance in unstable platform was analyzed concurrently with reaction time task (probe). Twenty-two participants (16 and 17 years old) performed the balance task during 10s and responded to the auditory stimulus. In IF, participants were instructed to direct their attention toward the feet movements and to keep them in a horizontal position. In EF, participants were instructed to direct their attention toward the bottom of the platform seeking to prevent it touching the ground. No difference was found in the performance of the balance task and reaction time task between IF and EF (P>0.05). These results were explained by the experience levels of the participants in the task.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2013
Paulo César Andrean; Bruno Secco Faquin; Juliana Bayeux Dascal; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
The present study analyzed the effect of interlateral transfer direction while learning a touching finger sequence task. Twenty right-handed participants (18-30 years old) were divided into two groups: practice with the right hand (GD) and left hand (GE). Groups were evaluated during pretest, posttest, and retention phases. The task was performing the higher number of finger touches between tomb and the other fingers, during 10 seconds, in a pre-stablished order. The GE showed higher number of finger touchs with the left hand in posttest (Z=2,2; P=0,028) compared to pretest. The GD increased the number of finger touches from pretest to retention for the right hand (Z=2,4; P=0,017) and left hand (Z=2,6; P=0,009), indicating learning and interlateral transfer of learning. The interlateral transfer of learning from the right to the left hand was explained by the principle of trainability.The present study analyzed the effect of interlateral transfer direction while learning a touching finger sequence task. Twenty right-handed participants (18-30 years old) were divided into two groups: practice with the right hand (GD) and left hand (GE). Groups were evaluated during pretest, posttest, and retention phases. The task was performing the higher number of finger touches between tomb and the other fingers, during 10 seconds, in a pre-stablished order. The GE showed higher number of finger touchs with the left hand in posttest (Z=2,2; P=0,028) compared to pretest. The GD increased the number of finger touches from pretest to retention for the right hand (Z=2,4; P=0,017) and left hand (Z=2,6; P=0,009), indicating learning and interlateral transfer of learning. The interlateral transfer of learning from the right to the left hand was explained by the principle of trainability.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2013
Paulo César Andrean; Bruno Secco Faquin; Juliana Bayeux Dascal; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
The present study analyzed the effect of interlateral transfer direction while learning a touching finger sequence task. Twenty right-handed participants (18-30 years old) were divided into two groups: practice with the right hand (GD) and left hand (GE). Groups were evaluated during pretest, posttest, and retention phases. The task was performing the higher number of finger touches between tomb and the other fingers, during 10 seconds, in a pre-stablished order. The GE showed higher number of finger touchs with the left hand in posttest (Z=2,2; P=0,028) compared to pretest. The GD increased the number of finger touches from pretest to retention for the right hand (Z=2,4; P=0,017) and left hand (Z=2,6; P=0,009), indicating learning and interlateral transfer of learning. The interlateral transfer of learning from the right to the left hand was explained by the principle of trainability.The present study analyzed the effect of interlateral transfer direction while learning a touching finger sequence task. Twenty right-handed participants (18-30 years old) were divided into two groups: practice with the right hand (GD) and left hand (GE). Groups were evaluated during pretest, posttest, and retention phases. The task was performing the higher number of finger touches between tomb and the other fingers, during 10 seconds, in a pre-stablished order. The GE showed higher number of finger touchs with the left hand in posttest (Z=2,2; P=0,028) compared to pretest. The GD increased the number of finger touches from pretest to retention for the right hand (Z=2,4; P=0,017) and left hand (Z=2,6; P=0,009), indicating learning and interlateral transfer of learning. The interlateral transfer of learning from the right to the left hand was explained by the principle of trainability.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2011
Giane Francine Garcia de Moraes; Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Bruno Secco Faquin; Tiago Gorgatti; Inara Marques; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
It was analyzed the athletic performance in basketball players using wheelchairs prescribed (specific) and not prescribed (non-specific) according to the particularity of each player. Six wheelchair basketball players performed the 20 meters speed test (with and without dribbling a ball), and the agility test without dribbling a ball. Scales of effort and comfort were used after each test. It was verified better performance (P<0,05) and comfort (P<0,05) in the tests performed with the use of the prescribed wheelchair. Therefore, it was suggested that the use of prescribed wheelchair specially designed for each athlete, for the optimization of their performance and comfort in the basketball wheelchair sport.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2011
Giane Francine Garcia de Moraes; Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Bruno Secco Faquin; Tiago Gorgatti; Inara Marques; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
It was analyzed the athletic performance in basketball players using wheelchairs prescribed (specific) and not prescribed (non-specific) according to the particularity of each player. Six wheelchair basketball players performed the 20 meters speed test (with and without dribbling a ball), and the agility test without dribbling a ball. Scales of effort and comfort were used after each test. It was verified better performance (P<0,05) and comfort (P<0,05) in the tests performed with the use of the prescribed wheelchair. Therefore, it was suggested that the use of prescribed wheelchair specially designed for each athlete, for the optimization of their performance and comfort in the basketball wheelchair sport.
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2015
Bruno Secco Faquin; Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Aurélio Grimble Izumi Gonçalves dos Santos; Tatiane Flávia de Oliveira; Alessandra Beggiato Porto; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
Revista Brasileira de Ciências do Esporte | 2015
Bruno Secco Faquin; Cristiane Regina Coelho Candido; Aurélio Grimble Izumi Gonçalves dos Santos; Tatiane Flávia de Oliveira; Alessandra Beggiato Porto; Victor Hugo Alves Okazaki
Collaboration
Dive into the Bruno Secco Faquin's collaboration.
Aurélio Grimble Izumi Gonçalves dos Santos
Universidade Estadual de Londrina
View shared research outputs