Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where C. Donald Heth is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by C. Donald Heth.


Journal of Comparative Psychology | 1998

PIGEONS' (COLUMBA LIVIA) ENCODING OF GEOMETRIC AND FEATURAL PROPERTIES OF A SPATIAL ENVIRONMENT

Debbie M. Kelly; Marcia L. Spetch; C. Donald Heth

Pigeons (Columba livia) searched for hidden food in a rectangular environment constructed to eliminate external orientation cues. A feature group was initially trained with distinct features in each corner. A geometric group was initially trained with no featural information. Tests revealed that both groups encoded the geometry of the apparatus. The geometric group was then retrained with features, and a series of tests was administered to both groups. Transformation tests revealed that the groups differed in reliance on features versus geometry. Pigeons in the feature group followed the positive feature even when it was placed in a geometrically incorrect corner, whereas pigeons in the geometric group showed shared control by features and geometry. Both groups were able to use features in corners distant to the goal to orient themselves, and both groups relied more on the color than on the shape of the features. Survival within an environment frequently requires efficient processing of spatial information. Spatial abilities underlie activities that are critical for the individual (e.g., establishment of lodging, avoidance of predation, and attainment of nourishment) and for a species (e.g., migratory behavior or reproduction); these activities may involve a


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes | 1976

Simultaneous and Backward Fear Conditioning as a Function of Number of CS-UCS Pairings.

C. Donald Heth

Four experiments examined the possibility that the outcome of simultaneous and backward fear conditioning procedures might depend upon the number of CS-UCS pairings. A punishment procedure will rats as subjects and shock as the UCS was used; the amount of suppression produced by response-contingent CS presentations indexed the strength of acquired fear. Experiments 1, 3, and 4 examined the suppressive tendencies of simultaneous-and backward-trained CSs after 0, 10, 20, 40, 80, and 160 pairings. The pattern of data suggested that initial pairings have the effect of increasing suppressive tendencies of the CS, while subsequent pairings decrease them. In addition, evidence of fear inhibition was found after 160 pairings in the case of the backward paradigm. Experiment 2 examined several nonassociative accounts based upon differential shock exposure. Groups given 10 pairings or 80 pairings were compared to ggroups given 10 pairings plus 70 shock-alone presentations. The results indicated that number of pairings, rather than number of UCS occurrences, is the important factor in decreasing the initial suppression. The evidence for eventual inhibition in the backward paradigm suggests that this occurs through the acquisition of inhibitory tendencies which are antagonistic to the previously conditioned excitation.


Memory & Cognition | 1994

Place recognition and way finding by children and adults

Edward H. Cornell; C. Donald Heth; Denise M. Alberts

Children and adults were escorted on their first walk across our university campus and were periodically led off the original route during the return trip. During the return, we stopped prior to intersections on and off the original route to obtain estimates of place recognition accuracy and confidence. The subjects were then asked to point to the path that led back to the start and were corrected if wrong. Accuracy of place recognition was intermediate in a way-finding task requiring reversal of an incidentally learned novel route. However, accuracy increased as subjects were farther from the original route, indicating that the presence of novel landmarks boosted the discrimination of old and new places. Eight-year-old children were less accurate than 12-year-old children and 25-year-old adults, who did not differ in accuracy. There was a similar age difference in the ability to point to the direction to return when subjects correctly recognized that they were off route. The results are used to develop a model of way finding by place recognition.


Developmental Psychology | 1989

Children's wayfinding: response to instructions to use environmental landmarks

Edward H. Cornell; C. Donald Heth; Lorri S. Broda

The paths taken by 6- and 12-year-old children who were asked to lead the way back after their first walk across a university campus were recorded. Prior to the return, children were either uninformed of the requirement to lead the way back, generally informed, or shown specific near or far landmarks during the walk. The results suggest a prudent prescription for parents who are concerned about their childs independent travel: Specify route features where the child should continue or change heading


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory | 1979

Response versus place learning by human infants.

Edward H. Cornell; C. Donald Heth

Two cross-setional studies examined how infants learn the location of visual events. In Experiment 1, infants of 4, 8, and 12 mo of age learned to turn one way to view a novel pattern. In a subsequent transfer task, they were rotated to face the opposite side of the room. The 4-mo-old infants tended to err by repeating their previously learned response, but within 16-20 trials their performance was comparable to the higher levels maintained by the older infants. These results suggest that young infants learn the location of the pattern primarily in terms of response cues, whereas older infants employ both response cues and place cues. Experiment 2 was designed to independently assess the use of response cues and place cues by infants of 4, 8, 12, and 16 mo of age. All infants were able to rapidly learn and remember the location of the novel pattern when they were given response cues. There was a gradual emergence of place-cue use associated with age. It is suggested that the decrease in infant egocentricity in such spatial localization tasks may in fact reflect an age-related increase in the variety of reliable cues responded to by infants.


Physiology & Behavior | 2001

Conditioned taste aversion induced by wheel running is not due to novelty of the wheel

C. Donald Heth; Peter Inglis; James C. Russell; W. David Pierce

Under a within-subjects design, food- and water-restricted rats showed a significant reduction in consumption of a flavor associated with the opportunity to run compared to another flavor associated with a novel wheel without the opportunity to run. Furthermore, there was no evidence that consumption of the flavor paired with the novel wheel differed from a home cage control.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 1983

Spatial cognition: Gathering strategies used by preschool children

Edward H. Cornell; C. Donald Heth

How preschool children retrieve hidden objects was examined in two cross-sectional studies. The first was a simple task in which 1- and 3-year-olds saw two treats hidden in their living room. The newly walking infants generally sought the closer treat first, providing evidence for a least-distance spatial strategy. However, this strategy was affected by a tendency to approach the hiding place most recently baited. Three-year-old children used a least-distance strategy regardless of the order of hidings. In Experiment 2, 3- and 5-year-olds saw 12 puzzle pieces hidden in various containers equally spaced within a naturally furnished childrens laboratory. Factors in addition to age were the distinctiveness of the containers and a requirement to return to the center of the array after each retrieval. Overall, children of both age groups were quite successful at this task, retrieving 11 of the pieces. However, 3-year-olds were less efficient, retrieving fewer pieces and requiring more searches. Detailed analyses of errors and patterns of choices indicated differential processes in achieving their performance. Three-year-old children showed the use of memories for events, discrimination of classes of hiding places, and efficient spatial biases. Five-year-old children were more likely to exhibit these processes concurrently.


Applied Cognitive Psychology | 1996

Serial Position Effects in Children's Route Reversal Errors: Implications for Police Search Operations

Edward H. Cornell; C. Donald Heth; Yvonne Kneubuhler; Sabena Sehgal

We illustrate how descriptions of way-finding behavior can be applied to the problem of searching for a lost child. We first establish a pattern of errors that children make when reversing a route. Eight- and 12-year-olds were escorted on a circular tour of a campus and then were asked to retrace the route from the end point to the beginning. Errors at intersections indicated that the original paths were more likely to be remembered at the beginning and end of the route than in the middle. Next, the obtained serial position effects were incorporated in an algorithm to estimate the probability that a child is within an area designated for search. The algorithm enhances the performance of a novice search manager in a simulation of an urban police search.


Obesity | 2007

Overeating by Young Obesity-prone and Lean Rats Caused by Tastes Associated With Low Energy Foods

W. David Pierce; C. Donald Heth; Joanna C. Owczarczyk; James C. Russell; Spencer D. Proctor

Objective: Childhood obesity is a prominent health problem that may involve early learning about tastes and the energy content of foods. We tested the hypothesis that food tastes predictive of low energy content cause overeating in young animals.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 1980

Three experiences affecting spatial discrimination learning by ambulatory children

C. Donald Heth; Edward H. Cornell

Abstract Children 1 and 3 years of age were given a two-choice spatial discrimination task. They were placed in front of a barrier and encouraged to walk around it; one route around the barrier was blocked. Three experiments studied the effects of different types of experience with this spatial problem. The first examined trial and error experience, in which the child was allowed to walk the route he or she had chosen. Although most children readily learned to choose the open route, the pattern of choices indicated that 3-year-olds were more likely to achieve a learning criterion after an error than 1-year-olds. A second experiment looked at the effects of showing the children the layout of the problem prior to trial and error experience. The 3-year-olds were again more likely than the 1-year-olds to achieve criterion after an error; furthermore, they were likely to know the correct route on the first trial. The third experiment let the children watch their parents take the correct route. This procedure eliminated age differences in performance. The initial choices of the 1-year-olds indicated that they were likely to know the correct route, and if they did not, they were just as likely to learn from their mistakes as the 3-year-olds. The data were interpreted within a mathematical model of learning. Based upon these analyses, we propose two developmental trends. One involves a growth in sensitivity to the consequences of a choice of route. The other is a progression from social learning to more independent and ideational methods of spatial problem solving.

Collaboration


Dive into the C. Donald Heth's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge