Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Carmel M. Loughland is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Carmel M. Loughland.


Current Opinion in Psychiatry | 2009

Emotional face processing in schizophrenia

Richard W. Morris; Cynthia Shannon Weickert; Carmel M. Loughland

PURPOSE OF REVIEW Recent studies of face processing deficits in people with schizophrenia have begun to shed light on the answers to four important questions: What is the major component of the deficit? Does it have a neurobiological basis? When does it appear in development? Can it be remediated? RECENT FINDINGS Eye contact is especially important for recognizing fearful expressions. Patients with schizophrenia avoid gazing at important facial feature regions (especially eyes) and are particularly impaired in recognizing fear. Patients were thought to exhibit decreased amygdala activity when viewing fearful faces; however, more recent studies have revealed limbic hyperactivity in patients when viewing nonfearful and fearful faces compared with baseline. Amygdala hyperactivity can also be detected when people with schizophrenia fail to appropriately recognize fear faces. Studies indicate that there are developmental changes in amygdala activation, including limbic hyperactivity to fear faces during adolescence, a critical time for the onset of schizophrenia. New treatment strategies can increase gaze to the eye region, which could improve emotional recognition in patients. SUMMARY These findings suggest that there may soon be more options for overcoming specific problems in emotional face evaluation in people who have schizophrenia.


Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry | 2010

Australian schizophrenia research bank: a database of comprehensive clinical, endophenotypic and genetic data for aetiological studies of schizophrenia

Carmel M. Loughland; Daren Draganic; Kathryn McCabe; Jacqueline Richards; Aslam Nasir; Joanne Allen; Stanley V. Catts; Assen Jablensky; Frans Henskens; Patricia T. Michie; Bryan J. Mowry; Christos Pantelis; Ulrich Schall; Rodney J. Scott; Paul A. Tooney; Vaughan J. Carr

Objective: This article describes the establishment of the Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank (ASRB), which operates to collect, store and distribute linked clinical, cognitive, neuroimaging and genetic data from a large sample of people with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Method: Recruitment sources for the schizophrenia sample include a multi-media national advertising campaign, inpatient and community treatment services and non-government support agencies. Healthy controls have been recruited primarily through multi-media advertisements. All participants undergo an extensive diagnostic and family history assessment, neuropsychological evaluation, and blood sample donation for genetic studies. Selected individuals also complete structural MRI scans. Results: Preliminary analyses of 493 schizophrenia cases and 293 healthy controls are reported. Mean age was 39.54 years (SD = 11.1) for the schizophrenia participants and 37.38 years (SD = 13.12) for healthy controls. Compared to the controls, features of the schizophrenia sample included a higher proportion of males (cases 65.9%; controls 46.8%), fewer living in married or de facto relationships (cases 16.1%; controls 53.6%) and fewer years of education (cases 13.05, SD = 2.84; controls 15.14, SD = 3.13), as well as lower current IQ (cases 102.68, SD = 15.51; controls 118.28, SD = 10.18). These and other sample characteristics are compared to those reported in another large Australian sample (i.e. the Low Prevalence Disorders Study), revealing some differences that reflect the different sampling methods of these two studies. Conclusion: The ASRB is a valuable and accessible schizophrenia research facility for use by approved scientific investigators. As recruitment continues, the approach to sampling for both cases and controls will need to be modified to ensure that the ASRB samples are as broadly representative as possible of all cases of schizophrenia and healthy controls.


Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2010

Visual scanning of faces in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: Attention to the mouth or the eyes?

Linda E. Campbell; Kathryn McCabe; Kate Leadbeater; Ulrich Schall; Carmel M. Loughland; Dominique Rich

Previous research demonstrates that people with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS) have social and interpersonal skill deficits. However, the basis of this deficit is unknown. This study examined, for the first time, how people with 22q11DS process emotional face stimuli using visual scanpath technology. The visual scanpaths of 17 adolescents and age/gender matched healthy controls were recorded while they viewed face images depicting one of seven basic emotions (happy, sad, surprised, angry, fear, disgust and neutral). Recognition accuracy was measured concurrently. People with 22q11DS differed significantly from controls, displaying visual scanpath patterns that were characterised by fewer fixations and a shorter scanpath length. The 22q11DS group also spent significantly more time gazing at the mouth region and significantly less time looking at eye regions of the faces. Recognition accuracy was correspondingly impaired, with 22q11DS subjects displaying particular deficits for fear and disgust. These findings suggest that 22q11DS is associated with a maladaptive visual information processing strategy that may underlie affect recognition accuracy and social functioning deficits in this group.


Journal of Psychophysiology | 2001

In search of the "Duchenne smile": Evidence from eye movements

Leanne M. Williams; Carl Senior; Anthony S. David; Carmel M. Loughland; Evian Gordon

Abstract Duchenne de Boulogne (1862/1990) observed that authentic smiles are distinguished from posed smiles by contractions of the orbicularis oculi muscle that surrounds the eye. Hager and Ekman (1985) subsequently reported that the “Duchenne smile” is discriminated specifically by contractions at the outer corners of the eyes (“crows feet” wrinkles). Little is known of the visuo-cognitive strategies that underpin perception of the Duchenne smile. We examined ocular dynamics (pattern of eye fixations) as an overt and accessible index of these strategies (Stark & Ellis, 1981). The spatio-temporal fixation indices were number and duration of foveal fixations to the crows feet area relative to the total face area. In line with predictions, subjects (n = 58) made proportionately more fixations of longer duration to the crows feet area for smiling, compared to negative (sad) and baseline (neutral) control expressions. It is suggested that a “hardwired” response may be involved in reference to this particular...


Psychopathology | 2003

The Five Symptom Dimensions and Depression in Schizophrenia

Kwang-Hyuk Lee; Anthony Harris; Carmel M. Loughland; Leanne M. Williams

The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between the five-factor model of psychopathology and depression in schizophrenia. Symptoms were rated using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and the Montgomery and Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) in 105 chronic patients with schizophrenia. Principal-component analysis (PCA) produced a five-factor solution for the PANSS (psychomotor poverty, disorganisation, reality distortion, excitement, and depression), and a two-factor solution for the MADRS (psychological and behavioural depression). The PANSS depression factor was highly associated with the MADRS psychological depression factor but not with MADRS behavioural depression. By contrast, the PANSS excitement factor showed a strong positive correlation with the behavioural depression factor but not with psychological depression. These MADRS factors were not associated significantly with the core PANSS factors, including psychomotor poverty. It is suggested that depression exists as an independent domain, differentiated from negative symptoms, in the structure of schizophrenia symptomatology.


Journal of Psychiatric Research | 2012

Relationship between childhood adversity and clinical and cognitive features in schizophrenia.

Kathryn McCabe; Elizabeth Maloney; Helen J. Stain; Carmel M. Loughland; Vaughan J. Carr

Childhood adversity is associated with elevated risk for a wide range of adult psychiatric disorders, and has significant and sustained negative effects on adult behavioural and social functioning. Elevated rates of childhood adversity have been reported for people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. The aim of the present study was to assess rates of retrospectively reported childhood adversity among adults with schizophrenia and to examine the relationship between childhood adversity and clinical and cognitive features. Data were available for 408 schizophrenia participants and 267 healthy control participants recruited through the Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank (ASRB). History of childhood adversity was obtained using the Childhood Adversity Questionnaire (CAQ). A five-factor solution was identified from the CAQ. Schizophrenia participants reported experiencing more childhood adversities than controls. In both groups, those reporting childhood adversity were more likely to be female and older. Among participants with schizophrenia, positive symptom severity and fewer years of education were associated with childhood adversity. Lower IQ scores and personality traits were associated with reporting a greater number of childhood adversities and with adversity sub-types of abusive, neglectful and dysfunctional parenting. The rate of childhood adversity reported in this sample was high which suggests greater exposure to adverse childhood events among participants with schizophrenia in comparison with healthy controls. We identified unique groups amongst CAQ items that provided a salient framework from which to investigate the connection between childhood adversity and clinical and cognitive features.


Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2001

Syndromes of schizophrenia and smooth-pursuit eye movement dysfunction

Kwang-Hyuk Lee; Leanne M. Williams; Carmel M. Loughland; Dean Davidson; Evian Gordon

There have been a number of studies on smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM) dysfunction in schizophrenia. However, the association between SPEM dysfunction and particular clinical symptoms remains unclear. We examined SPEM dysfunction in relation to schizophrenic symptoms using both the positive/negative dichotomy and the three-syndrome model. Subjects included 78 patients with schizophrenia and 60 healthy control subjects. SPEM performance was indexed by root mean square error. Symptom profiles were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), and the three-primary syndromes were identified by factor analysis of PANSS ratings (Psychomotor poverty: deficit negative symptoms; Disorganization: defined primarily by thought disorder; and Reality distortion: hallucinations and delusions). Compared with controls, the schizophrenia group showed significant impairment in global SPEM function. The three-syndrome approach produced more specific findings than the dichotomous model. Of the three syndromes, only the Disorganization dimension showed a significant association with increased global SPEM dysfunction. The specificity of SPEM dysfunction to Disorganization was verified in comparisons among schizophrenia subgroups and the control group. By contrast, the general domains of positive and negative symptoms were both found to be modestly associated with SPEM dysfunction. The separation of positive and negative symptoms that contribute to Disorganization from those that define Reality Distortion and Psychomotor Poverty has revealed significant new associations between SPEM and schizophrenic symptoms. These findings are interpreted in light of the proposal that the Disorganization syndrome is the central form of pathology in schizophrenia.


Schizophrenia Research | 2007

RBANS neuropsychological profiles within schizophrenia samples recruited from non-clinical settings

Carmel M. Loughland; Terry J. Lewin; Vaughan J. Carr; Jim Sheedy; Anthony Harris

BACKGROUND This paper examines the potential impact of recruitment source differences in schizophrenia research by comparing the neuropsychological performance of volunteers from the NISAD Schizophrenia Research Register with recently published schizophrenia normative data for the Repeatable Battery for Neuropsychological Status (RBANS). METHODS The Register sample comprised 285 volunteers with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Their RBANS performance was compared with US data from 575 predominantly outpatient-recruited schizophrenia patients. RESULTS The Register sample displayed impairments in immediate and delayed memory, but near-normal language, attention and visuospatial-constructional performance (mean RBANS total score=88.72, SD=16.35). By contrast, health service-recruited schizophrenia patients displayed impairments on all RBANS scales (mean RBANS total score=70.54, SD=14.80). Within the Register sample, volunteers with low levels of current functioning had immediate and delayed memory performance comparable to the US schizophrenia sample. Gender and school completion status were also associated with different RBANS profiles. CONCLUSIONS These findings reinforce the notion that a severity/functioning gradient exists across schizophrenia recruitment sources, which has important implications for research design and generalizability. Memory impairments have emerged as a central feature of schizophrenia.


Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry | 2001

The NISAD Schizophrenia Research Register: why do we need a database of schizophrenia volunteers?

Carmel M. Loughland; Vaughan J. Carr; Terry J. Lewin

Objective: This paper documents the establishment of the Schizophrenia Research Register of the Neuroscience Institute of Schizophrenia and Allied Disorders (NISAD). This register aims to provide a volunteer pool of people with a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia who are willing to consider participating in research projects. This unique resource is accessible to the general scientific research community. Method: The Register, which operates as a standalone, computerized relational database, maintains demographic and clinical information about individuals with schizophrenia recruited through media campaigns, and general health and non-government support agencies. Preliminary data are reported on the first 400 people with schizophrenia who registered on the database, together with selected comparisons with data from the national Low Prevalence (psychotic) Disorders Study (LPDS). Results: Individuals currently on the Register have a mean age of 38.74 years (SD = 11.41) and are predominantly Australian born (85.1%), which is consistent with data from the LPDS. However, the gender distribution is more balanced compared with the LPDS (53.8% vs 65.4% males) and proportionately more registrants are married or in de facto relationships (18.4% vs 10.8%). Registrants also tend to have lower current symptomatology and higher functioning relative to participants in the LPDS. Conclusions: The Register provides a unique and invaluable educational and research resource, as well as a complementary recruitment source for researchers who would otherwise rely on samples drawn primarily from mental health services.


Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2011

Visual scanpath abnormalities in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: is this a face specific deficit?

Kathryn McCabe; Dominique Rich; Carmel M. Loughland; Ulrich Schall; Linda E. Campbell

People with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS) have deficits in face emotion recognition. However, it is not known whether this is a deficit specific to faces, or represents maladaptive information processing strategies to complex stimuli in general. This study examined the specificity of face emotion processing deficits in 22q11DS by exploring recognition accuracy and visual scanpath performance to a Faces task compared to a Weather Scene task. Seventeen adolescents with 22q11DS (11=females, age=17.4) and 18 healthy controls (11=females, age=17.7) participated in the study. People with 22q11DS displayed an overall impoverished scanning strategy to face and weather stimuli alike, resulting in poorer accuracy across all stimuli for the 22q11DS participants compared to controls. While the control subjects altered their information processing in response to faces, a similar change was not present in the 22q11DS group indicating different visual scanpath strategies to identify category within each of the tasks, of which faces appear to represent a particularly difficult subcategory. To conclude, while this study indicates that people with 22q11DS have a general visual processing deficit, the lack of strategic change between tasks suggest that the 22q11DS group did not adapt to the change in stimuli content as well as the controls, indicative of cognitive inflexibility rather than a face specific deficit.

Collaboration


Dive into the Carmel M. Loughland's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Vaughan J. Carr

University of New South Wales

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Assen Jablensky

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bryan J. Mowry

University of Queensland

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge