Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Carol A. McCleary is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Carol A. McCleary.


Neurology | 2000

Estrogen for Alzheimer’s disease in women: Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial

Victor W. Henderson; Annlia Paganini-Hill; Bruce L. Miller; R. J. Elble; P. F. Reyes; Donna Shoupe; Carol A. McCleary; R. A. Klein; A. M. Hake; Martin R. Farlow

Background: AD, the most prevalent cause of dementia, affects twice as many women as men. Therapeutic options are limited, but results of prior studies support the hypothesis that estrogen treatment may improve symptoms of women with this disorder. Methods: Forty-two women with mild-to-moderate dementia due to AD were enrolled into a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group trial of unopposed conjugated equine estrogens (1.25 mg/day) for 16 weeks. Results: Outcome data were available for 40 women at 4 weeks and 36 women at 16 weeks. At both 4 and 16 weeks, there were no significant differences or statistical trends between treatment groups on the primary outcome measure (the cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale), clinician-rated global impression of change, or caregiver-rated functional status. Exploratory analyses of mood and specific aspects of cognitive performance also failed to demonstrate substantial group differences. Conclusions: Although conclusions are limited by small sample size and the possibility of a type II error, results suggest that short-term estrogen therapy does not improve symptoms of most women with AD. These findings do not address possible long-term effects of estrogen in AD, possible interactions between estrogen and other treatment modalities, or putative effects of estrogen in preventing or delaying onset of this disorder.


Psychoneuroendocrinology | 1999

Pregnancy, the postpartum, and steroid hormones: effects on cognition and mood

J. Galen Buckwalter; Frank Z. Stanczyk; Carol A. McCleary; Brendon W. Bluestein; Deborah K. Buckwalter; Katherine P. Rankin; Lilly Chang; T. Murphy Goodwin

The effects of pregnancy on cognition and mood were examined using a repeated-measures design. Nineteen women, average age 33, were tested with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery during their last 2 months of pregnancy and again within 2 months of delivery. Blood samples were obtained from all subjects and assayed for a variety of steroid hormones implicated in cognitive and mood functioning. Most participants also completed several self-report measures of mood. In comparison with performance after delivery, women showed significantly more impairment in aspects of verbal memory during pregnancy and also tended to report more negative mood states. Memory deficits were not explained by mood disturbances. No hormone assayed consistently related to cognitive performance during pregnancy. During pregnancy, higher levels of progesterone (P) were associated with greater mood disturbances and higher levels of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) with better mood. After delivery, testosterone (T) was strongly and consistently associated with greater reported mood disturbances. Our results confirm a peripartal memory deficit, which cannot be explained by the dramatic rise in circulating steroid hormones, or by mood status during pregnancy. Steroidal hormones, namely P, DHEA and T, appear to play a role in mood disturbances during, and after, pregnancy. Studies beginning earlier in pregnancy and continuing for an extended period of time after delivery are needed to confirm and expand these observations.


Neurology | 2000

Associations between circulating sex steroid hormones and cognition in normal elderly women

E.B. Drake; Victor W. Henderson; Frank Z. Stanczyk; Carol A. McCleary; Warren S. Brown; C.A. Smith; Rizzo Aa; G.A. Murdock; John Galen Buckwalter

Objective: To provide exploratory analyses of associations between levels of several sex hormones and cognitive performance in elderly women. Background: Sex steroid hormones are implicated in the cognitive processes of the adult brain. Comparing cognitive performance across or between conditions associated with different hormone levels, such as phases of the menstrual cycle, surgical menopause, and estrogen replacement therapy suggests conditions with higher levels of estrogen are associated with better verbal memory and possibly worse visuospatial ability. Method: The authors measured circulating sex hormone levels in 39 highly educated, nondemented, predominantly white elderly women. Levels were correlated with neuropsychological performance, controlling for age, education, frequency of prior testing, use of estrogen replacement, and depression. Results: High estradiol levels were associated with better delayed verbal memory and retrieval efficiency, whereas low levels were associated with better immediate and delayed visual memory. Levels of testosterone were related positively to verbal fluency. Levels of progesterone and androstenedione were unrelated to cognitive performance. Conclusions: Both estrogen and testosterone showed associations with cognitive performance. Estrogen may enhance, and depress, specific cognitive skills.


Brain Injury | 1998

Depression, cognition, and functional correlates of recovery outcome after traumatic brain injury

Paul Satz; David Forney; Kenneth Zaucha; Robert R. Asarnow; Roger Light; Carol A. McCleary; Harvey S. Levin; Daniel F. Kelly; Marvin Bergsneider; David A. Hovda; Neil A. Martin; Normal Namerow; Donald P. Becker

The present study investigated the prevalence and magnitude of depressive symptomatology in a sample of patients who had sustained traumatic brain injury (TBI) six months earlier. Depression was examined as a function of recovery outcome status, and its association with neuropsychological functioning, personal competency, and employability was also explored. Subjects were 100 patients who had previously sustained moderate-to-severe TBI who were enrolled as research subjects in the UCLA Brain Injury Research Center, and 30 matched control subjects who had sustained traumatic injuries other than to the head six months prior to evaluation. The results showed a significant association between depression and recovery status as measured by the Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS). A significant majority of depressed subjects were found in the poorer GOS outcome groups (severe and moderate disability), compared to TBI subjects who had good GOS outcomes, and control subjects. This association was also reflected in the magnitude of the mean depression scores on two self-report measures of depression. However, no association was found between depression status and performance on the neuropsychological measures. Effects of depression were found only on an examiner-rated Patient Competency scale, and a metacognition measure based on self-report. These results are discussed in terms of brain injury severity, recovery status, and metacognition issues in TBI and other disorders.


American Journal of Alzheimers Disease and Other Dementias | 2009

Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer's disease.

Linda J. Clark; Margaret Gatz; Ling Zheng; Yu-Ling Chen; Carol A. McCleary; Wendy J. Mack

Background: Few longitudinal studies evaluate differences in patterns of change of category compared to letter fluency across the spectrum of cognitive impairment. Methods: We compared change in category (animal and supermarket) and letter (F, A, S) fluency among 239 participants in 3 groups: remained cognitively normal throughout follow-up (n = 96), developed Alzheimer’s Disease (AD; preclinical AD, n = 21), and with AD at initial testing (prevalent AD, n = 122). Results: At baseline, prevalent and preclinical AD groups scored lower on animal than letter fluency. On all fluency measures, the prevalent AD declined faster than other groups (all P < .0001), and preclinical AD declined faster than unimpaired (all P ≤ .02). Overall, animal fluency declined faster than letter fluency; animal fluency declined significantly faster than letter fluency among cognitively normal and prevalent AD participants. Conclusion: Greater longitudinal declines in category compared to letter fluency are consistent with cross-sectional studies. Steeper declines on both fluency measures distinguish preclinical AD from cognitively unimpaired individuals.


Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology | 2000

Anosognosia and Alzheimer's Disease: The Role of Depressive Symptoms in Mediating Impaired Insight

C.A. Smith; Victor W. Henderson; Carol A. McCleary; G.A. Murdock; J. G. Buckwalter

The relation between anosognosia and dementia severity in Alzheimers disease (AD) has been unclear. We constructed a measure that quantified the difference between the perceptions of deficits of patients with AD (n = 23) and ratings from a knowledgeable informant as a measure of anosognosia. There was no correlation between dementia severity and anosognosia. However, dementia severity was positively correlated with the degree of anosognosia after controlling for depressive symptomatology (p = .03). Post-hoc analyses, also controlling for depressive symptoms, indicated that higher levels of anosognosia were associated with lower performance on specific cognitive tasks. These results suggest depressive symptoms may confound the relationship between anosognosia and dementia severity.


PLOS ONE | 2009

Transcriptome analysis of synaptoneurosomes identifies neuroplasticity genes overexpressed in incipient Alzheimer's disease.

Celia Williams; Ruty Mehrian Shai; Yongchun Wu; Ya Hsuan Hsu; Traci Sitzer; Bryan M. Spann; Carol A. McCleary; Yi Mo; Carol A. Miller

In Alzheimers disease (AD), early deficits in learning and memory are a consequence of synaptic modification induced by toxic beta-amyloid oligomers (oAβ). To identify immediate molecular targets downstream of oAβ binding, we prepared synaptoneurosomes from prefrontal cortex of control and incipient AD (IAD) patients, and isolated mRNAs for comparison of gene expression. This novel approach concentrates synaptic mRNA, thereby increasing the ratio of synaptic to somal mRNA and allowing discrimination of expression changes in synaptically localized genes. In IAD patients, global measures of cognition declined with increasing levels of dimeric Aβ (dAβ). These patients also showed increased expression of neuroplasticity related genes, many encoding 3′UTR consensus sequences that regulate translation in the synapse. An increase in mRNA encoding the GluR2 subunit of the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid receptor (AMPAR) was paralleled by elevated expression of the corresponding protein in IAD. These results imply a functional impact on synaptic transmission as GluR2, if inserted, maintains the receptors in a low conductance state. Some overexpressed genes may induce early deficits in cognition and others compensatory mechanisms, providing targets for intervention to moderate the response to dAβ.


Neurology | 2012

Long-term soy isoflavone supplementation and cognition in women A randomized, controlled trial

Victor W. Henderson; J.A. St. John; Howard N. Hodis; Naoko Kono; Carol A. McCleary; Adrian A. Franke; Wendy J. Mack

Objective: To determine the cognitive effects of long-term dietary soy isoflavones in a daily dose comparable to that of traditional Asian diets. Methods: In the double-blind Womens Isoflavone Soy Health trial, healthy postmenopausal women were randomly allocated to receive daily 25 g of isoflavone-rich soy protein (91 mg of aglycone weight of isoflavones: 52 mg of genistein, 36 mg of daidzein, and 3 mg glycitein) or milk protein−matched placebo. The primary cognitive endpoint compared between groups at 2.5 years was change from baseline on global cognition, a composite of the weighted sum of 14 neuropsychological test score changes. Secondary outcomes compared changes in cognitive factors and individual tests. Results: A total of 350 healthy postmenopausal women aged 45–92 years enrolled in this trial; 313 women with baseline and endpoint cognitive test data were included in intention-to-treat analyses. Adherence in both groups was nearly 90%. There was no significant between-group difference on change from baseline in global cognition (mean standardized improvement of 0.42 in the isoflavone group and 0.31 in the placebo group; mean standardized difference 0.11, 95% confidence interval [CI] −0.13 to 0.35). Secondary analyses indicated greater improvement on a visual memory factor in the isoflavone group (mean standardized difference 0.33, 95% CI 0.06–0.60) but no significant between-group differences on 3 other cognitive factors or individual test scores, and no significant difference within a subgroup of younger postmenopausal women. Conclusion: For healthy postmenopausal women, long-term dietary soy isoflavone supplementation in a dose comparable to that of traditional Asian diets has no effect on global cognition but may improve visual memory. Classification of evidence: This study provides Class I evidence that long-term dietary supplementation with isoflavone-rich soy protein does not improve global cognition of healthy postmenopausal women.


Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition | 2008

Metabolic syndrome and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged and older adults without diabetes.

Nicole M. Gatto; Victor W. Henderson; Jan A. St. John; Carol A. McCleary; Howard N. Hodis; Wendy J. Mack

ABSTRACT Objective: Few studies have addressed whether the metabolic syndrome (MetS) and its individual components are associated with cognitive function in middle-aged and older populations, as well as whether specific areas of cognition are more affected than others. We examined the cross-sectional association between MetS and six areas of cognitive function in healthy cognitively intact adults without diabetes (n = 853, mean age 61 years) randomized in two intervention trials. Methods: The National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) criteria were used to identify subjects with MetS. Cognitive function was assessed with a neuropsychological battery. A principal components analysis was used to extract five uncorrelated factors interpreted to represent five areas of cognition, and a measure of global cognition was calculated. Results: MetS was weakly but non-significantly associated with lower verbal learning (β = −.14 [SE(β) = 0.09], p = .15). As the number of MetS criteria increased, scores on global cognition (p trend = .01), verbal learning (p trend = .06) and semantic memory (p trend = .04) decreased. Hypertension was the only MetS risk factor that was independently correlated with lower verbal learning (β = −.17 [SE(β)  = 0.08], p = .04), semantic memory (β = −.26 [SE(β) = 0.08], p = .001) and global cognition (β = −.15 [SE(β) = 0.07], p = .04). Conclusion: This study adds to the evidence of an association between MetS and lower cognitive function among healthy middle-aged and older adults without CVD and diabetes, as well as confirms the correlation between hypertension and lower cognition.


Neurology | 2016

Cognitive effects of estradiol after menopause A randomized trial of the timing hypothesis

Victor W. Henderson; Jan A. St. John; Howard N. Hodis; Carol A. McCleary; Frank Z. Stanczyk; Donna Shoupe; Naoko Kono; Laurie Dustin; Hooman Allayee; Wendy J. Mack

Objective: To test the hypothesis that effects of estrogen-containing hormone therapy on cognitive abilities differ between postmenopausal women near to, and further from, menopause. Methods: In this randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial, healthy women within 6 years of menopause or 10+ years after menopause were randomly assigned to oral 17β-estradiol 1 mg/d or placebo. Women with a uterus received cyclic micronized progesterone vaginal gel or placebo. The primary outcome assessed at 2.5 and 5 years, compared between treatment groups, was change in a standardized composite of neuropsychological test scores assessing verbal episodic memory. Secondary outcomes assessed executive functions and global cognition. Results: A total of 567 women were included in modified intention-to-treat analyses after a mean treatment duration of 57 months. For verbal memory, the mean estradiol minus placebo standardized difference in composite scores (−0.06, 95% confidence interval −0.22 to 0.09) was not significant (2-tailed p = 0.33). Differences were similar in early and late postmenopause groups (2-tailed interaction p = 0.88). Interactions between postmenopause groups and differences between treatment groups were not significant for executive functions or global cognition. Conclusions: Estradiol initiated within 6 years of menopause does not affect verbal memory, executive functions, or global cognition differently than therapy begun 10+ years after menopause. Estradiol neither benefits nor harms these cognitive abilities regardless of time since menopause. Classification of evidence: This study provides Class I evidence that estradiol initiated within 6 years of menopause does not affect cognition at 2.5 years differently than estradiol initiated 10+ years after menopause.

Collaboration


Dive into the Carol A. McCleary's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Wendy J. Mack

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Howard N. Hodis

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jan A. St. John

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Naoko Kono

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Frank Z. Stanczyk

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Paul Satz

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Roger Light

Memorial Hospital of South Bend

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Donna Shoupe

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Margaret Gatz

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge