Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Carolyn Zahn-Waxler is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Carolyn Zahn-Waxler.


Developmental Psychology | 1992

Development of concern for others

Carolyn Zahn-Waxler; Marian Radke-Yarrow; Elizabeth Wagner; Michael Chapman

The development of prosocial and reparative bebaviors was investigated by examining childrens responses to distresses they caused and those they witnessed in others during the 2nd year of life. Prosocial behaviors (help, sharing, provision of comfort) emerged between the ages of 1 and 2, increasing in frequency and variety over this time period. These behaviors were linked to expressions of concern as well as efforts to understand and experience the others plight. Childrens reparative behaviors after they had caused distress also increased with age. Age changes in these early signs of moral development were accompanied by social-cognitive changes in self-recognition. In assessments at age 2, children were most responsive to distress in their mothers but also showed some sensitivity toward unfamiliar persons


Annual Review of Clinical Psychology | 2008

Disorders of Childhood and Adolescence: Gender and Psychopathology

Carolyn Zahn-Waxler; Elizabeth A. Shirtcliff; Kristine Marceau

Early-onset disorders (e.g., conduct problems, autism) show a marked male preponderance, whereas adolescent-onset disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety) show a marked female preponderance. A developmental psychopathology framework provides a means to investigate complex gender-related etiologies of these different disorders. This review focuses on biological and environmental factors implicated in the development of conduct problems and depression in boys and girls. Boys and girls showed certain differences in types, rates, comorbidities, antecedents, correlates, and trajectories of these problems. Origins of male and female preponderant problems are likely to be rooted, in part, in biological, physical, cognitive, and social-emotional differences in boys and girls that can precede the expression of clinical problems. These male-like and female-like characteristics are considered regarding conduct problems and depression to explore how they inform biological and environmental theories about gender and psychopathology. At the same time, because boys and girls also show many similarities, it is important to avoid sex-stereotyping mental health problems.


Development and Psychopathology | 2003

The development of psychopathology in females and males: current progress and future challenges.

Nicki R. Crick; Carolyn Zahn-Waxler

In recent years, the role of gender in the development of psychopathology has increasingly attracted the attention of researchers, theoreticians, and other professionals interested in the well-being of children and adolescents. This interest has taken diverse forms, ranging from the examination of sex differences in the prevalence of adjustment difficulties to the exploration of unique etiologies and trajectories in the development of psychopathology for boys versus girls. In this paper we (a) critically examine the current status of available theories, research, and methods related to the study of gender and psychopathology and provide recommendations for future work; (b) identify promising new trends that appear to have utility for enhancing our understanding of the role of gender in the development of adjustment difficulties; and (c) generate conclusions regarding gender and psychopathology by integrating information from past and present work with new ideas about fruitful directions for future inquiry.


Child Development | 1986

Learning to Talk about Emotions: A Functionalist Perspective.

Inge Bretherton; Janet J. Fritz; Carolyn Zahn-Waxler; Doreen Ridgeway

BRETHERTON, INGE; FRITZ, JANET; ZAHN-WAXLER, CAROLYN; and RIDGEWAY, DOREEN. Learning to Talk about Emotions: A Functionalist Perspective. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1986, 57, 529-548. Although the recent focus on functionalist theories of emotions has led to an upsurge of interest in many aspects of emotional development, not enough attention has been paid to young childrens developing ability to talk about emotions. In this paper we attempt to place what is presently known about this topic into a framework that emphasizes the intrapsychic and interpersonal functions of emotion. We also consider suggestive evidence concerning the importance of the ability to talk about emotions in the conduct of interpersonal interaction. The paper concludes with some ideas on future directions for research, placing particular emphasis on a functionalist approach to the analysis of emotion-denoting terms.


Development and Psychopathology | 1993

Warriors and worriers: Gender and psychopathology

Carolyn Zahn-Waxler

Antisocial behaviors in females may differ from more prototypically “male” patterns of aggression, violence, and criminality that dominate criteria for conduct problems in diagnostic systems. This raises questions of how to define and investigate conduct problems in females as well as how to identify differential childhood antecedents. A developmental psychopathology perspective is advanced as one useful approach to understanding adaptive and maladaptive social patterns in males and females that may lead to different developmental trajectories and influence the forms of psychopathology that develop. The utility of de-emphasizing serious physical aggression as one important criteria for conduct problems is questioned. Recommendations that particular forms of deviance in females be considered as markers of their antisocial patterns (e.g., somatic complaints, friendlessness, underachievement) are also called into question. Rather than adopt sex-specific criteria to assess conduct problems, it is necessary to expand and broaden the diagnostic categories to include serious externalizing problems regardless of whether they occur in males or females.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology | 1996

Individual differences in emotion regulation and behavior problems in preschool children.

Pamela M. Cole; Carolyn Zahn-Waxler; Nathan A. Fox; Barbara A. Usher; Jean Darby Welsh

Emotion regulation (ER) was assessed during a negative mood induction in 79 preschoolers who varied in degree of behavior problems. Facial expressivity during the induction was used to identify 3 ER groups: inexpressive, modulated expressive, and highly expressive. Group differences in ER were significantly related to heart rate and skin conductance. Inexpressive preschoolers had the highest heart rate, lowest vagal tone, and smallest autonomic nervous system (ANS) change during the induction. Highly expressive preschoolers had the slowest heart rate, highest vagal tone, and largest ANS change. The inexpressive and highly expressive groups had more externalizing symptoms than the modulated group at preschool age and at follow-up at the end of 1st grade. Inexpressive preschoolers appeared to have more depressed and anxious symptoms at follow-up.


Development and Psychopathology | 1990

Antecedents of problem behaviors in children of depressed mothers

Carolyn Zahn-Waxler; Ronald J. Iannotti; E. Mark Cummings; Susan Denham

Aggression observed in 2-year-old children of well and depressed mothers was examined in relation to problem behaviors at ages 5–6. Both normative (e.g., object struggles, rough play) and maladaptive (e.g., dysregulated, out-of-control behavior) forms of toddler aggression were identified. Dysregulated aggression predicted (a) externalizing problems reported by mothers when children were 5 years old, and (b) childrens reports of difficulties during a structured psychiatric interview at age 6. Problems were more frequent and continuity patterns more evident in children of depressed, than well, mothers. Early maladaptive aggression was a better predictor of later externalizing, than internalizing problems. Childrearing practices of mothers of toddlers also appeared to contribute to later outcomes: negative influences were evident but protective patterns were present as well. Depressed mothers who used proactive childrearing approaches (e.g., anticipating the childs point of view; exerting modulated, respectful control; providing structure and organization during play environment) had children who showed fewer externalizing problems 3 years later.


Child Development | 1992

Temperament, Emotion, and Cognition at Fourteen Months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study

Robert N. Emde; Robert Plomin; JoAnn Robinson; Robin P. Corley; John C. DeFries; David W. Fulker; J. Steven Reznick; Joseph J. Campos; Jerome Kagan; Carolyn Zahn-Waxler

200 pairs of twins were assessed at 14 months of age in the laboratory and home. Measures were obtained of temperament, emotion, and cognition/language. Comparisons between identical and fraternal twin correlations suggest that individual differences are due in part to heritable influences. For temperament, genetic influence was significant for behavioral observations of inhibition to the unfamiliar, tester ratings of activity, and parental ratings of temperament. For emotion, significant genetic influence was found for empathy and parental ratings of negative emotion. The estimate of heritability for parental report of expression of negative emotions was relatively high, whereas that for expression of positive emotions was low, a finding consistent with previous research. For cognition and language, genetic influence was significant for behavioral indices of spatial memory, categorization, and word comprehension. Shared rearing environment appears influential for parental reports of language and for positive emotions, but not for other measures of emotion or for temperament.


Archive | 1991

The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Guilt and empathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of depression

Carolyn Zahn-Waxler; Pamela M. Cole; Karen Caplovitz Barrett

Adult depression is typified by prolonged episodes of sadness and inability to experience pleasure. There are different types of depression with a variety of causes as well as physical, cognitive, and affective symptoms. Physical symptoms include disturbances in activity, sleep, and eating patterns. Affective and cognitive symptoms include passivity, confusion, pessimism, helplessness, worthlessness, self-blame, and guilt. There are also different models of depression that tend to emphasize specific symptoms. The biological models, for example, focus on vegetative signs, biochemical changes, and brain-behavior pathways that are involved in depression. The cognitive and psychodynamic theories are based on reasoning, beliefs, and mood. Reformulated attribution theory (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) characterizes depression as having internal, stable, and global self-attributions of responsibility for negative events. Depressed individuals feel powerless yet responsible for events that go wrong, and their guilt is exaggerated. As Freud described it, depression is a disorder characterized by dissatisfaction with the ego on moral grounds. The guilt, shame, and self-derision that commonly accompany depression are viewed in biological models as correlates or outcomes of depression. In attribution theories, these qualities of individuals are viewed as proximal antecedents of depression. Traditional and reformulated psychodynamic theories view guilt, shame, and self-derision as both distal antecedents and central elements of the disorder.


Behavioral Sciences & The Law | 2009

Neurobiology of empathy and callousness: implications for the development of antisocial behavior.

Elizabeth A. Shirtcliff; Michael J. Vitacco; Alexander R. Graf; J B A Andrew Gostisha; L B A Jenna Merz; Carolyn Zahn-Waxler

Information on the neurobiology of empathy and callousness provides clinicians with an opportunity to develop sophisticated understanding of mechanisms underpinning antisocial behavior and its counterpart, moral decision-making. This article provides an integrated in-depth review of hormones (e.g. peripheral steroid hormones such as cortisol) and brain structures (e.g. insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and amygdala) implicated in empathy, callousness, and psychopathic-like behavior. The overarching goal of this article is to relate these hormones and brain structures to moral decision-making. This review will begin in the brain, but will then integrate information about biological functioning in the body, specifically stress-reactivity. Our aim is to integrate understanding of neural processes with hormones such as cortisol, both of which have demonstrated relationships to empathy, psychopathy, and antisocial behavior. The review proposes that neurobiological impairments in individuals who display little empathy are not necessarily due to a reduced ability to understand the emotions of others. Instead, evidence suggests that individuals who show little arousal to the distress of others likewise show decreased physiological arousal to their own distress; one manifestation of reduced stress reactivity may be a dysfunction in empathy, which supports psychopathic-like constructs (e.g. callousness). This integration will assist in the development of objective methodologies that can inform and monitor treatment interventions focused on decreasing antisocial behavior.

Collaboration


Dive into the Carolyn Zahn-Waxler's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Pamela M. Cole

Pennsylvania State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marian Radke-Yarrow

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bonnie Klimes-Dougan

The Catholic University of America

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

JoAnn Robinson

University of Connecticut

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robin P. Corley

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

E. Mark Cummings

University of West Virginia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Barbara A. Usher

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge