Chandani Nekitsing
University of Leeds
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Chandani Nekitsing.
PLOS ONE | 2014
Samantha J. Caton; Pam Blundell; Sara M. Ahern; Chandani Nekitsing; Annemarie Olsen; Per Møller; Helene Hausner; Eloı̈se Remy; Sophie Nicklaus; Claire Chabanet; Sylvie Issanchou; Marion M. Hetherington
Vegetable intake is generally low among children, who appear to be especially fussy during the pre-school years. Repeated exposure is known to enhance intake of a novel vegetable in early life but individual differences in response to familiarisation have emerged from recent studies. In order to understand the factors which predict different responses to repeated exposure, data from the same experiment conducted in three groups of children from three countries (n = 332) aged 4–38 m (18.9±9.9 m) were combined and modelled. During the intervention period each child was given between 5 and 10 exposures to a novel vegetable (artichoke puree) in one of three versions (basic, sweet or added energy). Intake of basic artichoke puree was measured both before and after the exposure period. Overall, younger children consumed more artichoke than older children. Four distinct patterns of eating behaviour during the exposure period were defined. Most children were “learners” (40%) who increased intake over time. 21% consumed more than 75% of what was offered each time and were labelled “plate-clearers”. 16% were considered “non-eaters” eating less than 10 g by the 5th exposure and the remainder were classified as “others” (23%) since their pattern was highly variable. Age was a significant predictor of eating pattern, with older pre-school children more likely to be non-eaters. Plate-clearers had higher enjoyment of food and lower satiety responsiveness than non-eaters who scored highest on food fussiness. Children in the added energy condition showed the smallest change in intake over time, compared to those in the basic or sweetened artichoke condition. Clearly whilst repeated exposure familiarises children with a novel food, alternative strategies that focus on encouraging initial tastes of the target food might be needed for the fussier and older pre-school children.
Appetite | 2015
Marion M. Hetherington; Camille Schwartz; J. Madrelle; Fiona Croden; Chandani Nekitsing; Carel Vereijken; H. Weenen
Breastfeeding (BF) is associated with willingness to accept vegetables. This may be due to the variety of flavours delivered via breast milk. Some mothers add vegetables to milk during complementary feeding (CF) to enhance acceptance. The present study tested a step-by-step exposure to vegetables in milk then rice during CF, on intake and liking of vegetables. Just before CF, enrolled mothers were randomised to an intervention (IG, n = 18; 6 BF) or control group (CG, n = 18; 6 BF). IG infants received 12 daily exposures to vegetable puree added to milk (days 1-12), then 12 × 2 daily exposures to vegetable puree added to rice at home (days 13-24). Plain milk and rice were given to CG. Then both received 11 daily exposures to vegetable puree. Intake was weighed and liking rated on days 25-26 and 33-35 after the start of CF in the laboratory, supplemented by the same data recorded at home. Vegetables were rotated daily (carrots, green beans, spinach, broccoli). Intake, liking and pace of eating were greater for IG than CG infants. Intake and liking of carrots were greater than green beans. However, at 6m then 18m follow up, vegetable (carrot > green beans) but not group differences were observed. Mothers reported appreciation of the structure and guidance of this systematic approach. Early exposure to vegetables in a step-by-step method could be included in CF guidelines and longer term benefits assessed by extending the exposure period.
Current obesity reports | 2018
Chandani Nekitsing; Marion M. Hetherington; Pam Blundell-Birtill
Purpose of ReviewThe present review was undertaken in order to summarize and evaluate recent research investigating taste exposure, sensory learning, and nutrition education interventions for promoting vegetable intake in preschool children.Recent FindingsOverall, taste exposure interventions yielded the best outcomes for increasing vegetable intake in early childhood. Evidence from sensory learning strategies such as visual exposure and experiential learning also show some success. While nutrition education remains the most common approach used in preschool settings, additional elements are needed to strengthen the educational program for increasing vegetable intake. There is a substantial gap in the evidence base to promote vegetable intake in food fussy children.SummaryThe present review reveals the relative importance of different intervention strategies for promoting vegetable intake. To strengthen intervention effects for improving vegetable intake in preschool children, future research could consider integrating taste exposure and sensory learning strategies with nutrition education within the preschool curriculum.
Appetite | 2018
Chandani Nekitsing; Pam Blundell-Birtill; Jennie E. Cockroft; Marion M. Hetherington
BACKGROUND Most children do not meet daily recommendations for fruit and vegetable intake, and consumption of vegetables remains especially low. Eating habits track from childhood to adulthood hence establishing liking and intake of vegetables is important. OBJECTIVE To identify the most successful strategies to enhance vegetable intake in preschool children aged 2-5 years. DESIGN The research was a systematic review and a meta-analysis of published studies. A comprehensive search strategy was performed using key databases such as Medline, Embase, PsychINFO, EBSCO and CENTRAL. Articles published between 2005-January 2016, specifically with measured vegetable consumption were included. RESULTS 30 articles and 44 intervention arms were identified for inclusion (n = 4017). Nine dominant intervention strategies emerged to promote vegetable intake in preschool children. These included; choice, pairing (stealth), education, food service, modelling, reward, taste exposure, variety and visual presentation. The meta-analysis revealed that interventions implementing repeated taste exposure had better pooled effects than those which did not. Intake increased with number of taste exposures and intake was greater when vegetables offered were in their plain form rather than paired with a flavor, dip or added energy (e.g. oil). Moreover, intake of vegetables which were unfamiliar/disliked increased more than those which were familiar/liked. CONCLUSIONS Repeated taste exposure is a simple technique that could be implemented in childcare settings and at home by parents. Health policy could specifically target the use of novel and disliked vegetables in childcare settings with emphasis on a minimum 8-10 exposures. The systematic review protocol was registered on the PROSPERO (number: CRD42016033984).
Appetite | 2018
Emma M. Krop; Marion M. Hetherington; Chandani Nekitsing; Sophie Miquel; Luminita Postelnicu; Anwesha Sarkar
Food Quality and Preference | 2016
Marion M. Hetherington; J. Madrelle; Chandani Nekitsing; C. Barends; C. de Graaf; S. Morgan; H. Parrott; H. Weenen
Food Quality and Preference | 2016
Chandani Nekitsing; J. Madrelle; C. Barends; C. de Graaf; H. Parrott; S. Morgan; H. Weenen; Marion M. Hetherington
Social Science & Medicine | 2017
Mark Conner; Tracy Sandberg; Chandani Nekitsing; Russell R. C. Hutter; Chantelle Wood; Cath Jackson; Gaston Godin; Paschal Sheeran
Appetite | 2018
Chandani Nekitsing; Pam Blundell-Birtill; Jennie E. Cockroft; Marion M. Hetherington
Appetite | 2018
Chandani Nekitsing; Pam Blundell-Birtill; Jennie E. Cockroft; Marion M. Hetherington