Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Chantal Kemner is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Chantal Kemner.


American Journal of Human Genetics | 2001

A genomewide screen for autism: Strong evidence for linkage to chromosomes 2q, 7q, and 16p

Sarah Palferman; Nicola Matthews; Martha Turner; Janette Moore; Amaia Hervas; Anne Aubin; Simon Wallace; Janine Michelotti; Catherine Wainhouse; Alina Paul; Elaine Thompson; Ramyani Gupta; Claire Garner; Marianne Murin; Christine M. Freitag; N Ryder; E Cottington; Jeremy R. Parr; Andrew Pickles; Michael Rutter; Anthony J. Bailey; Gabrielle Barnby; J A Lamb; Angela J. Marlow; Pat Scudder; Anthony P. Monaco; Gillian Baird; Antony Cox; Zoe Docherty; Pamela Warburton

Autism is characterized by impairments in reciprocal communication and social interaction and by repetitive and stereotyped patterns of activities and interests. Evidence for a strong underlying genetic predisposition comes from twin and family studies, although susceptibility genes have not yet been identified. A whole-genome screen for linkage, using 83 sib pairs with autism, has been completed, and 119 markers have been genotyped in 13 candidate regions in a further 69 sib pairs. The addition of new families and markers provides further support for previous reports of linkages on chromosomes 7q and 16p. Two new regions of linkage have also been identified on chromosomes 2q and 17q. The most significant finding was a multipoint maximum LOD score (MLS) of 3.74 at marker D2S2188 on chromosome 2; this MLS increased to 4.80 when only sib pairs fulfilling strict diagnostic criteria were included. The susceptibility region on chromosome 7 was the next most significant, generating a multipoint MLS of 3.20 at marker D7S477. Chromosome 16 generated a multipoint MLS of 2.93 at D16S3102, whereas chromosome 17 generated a multipoint MLS of 2.34 at HTTINT2. With the addition of new families, there was no increased allele sharing at a number of other loci originally showing some evidence of linkage. These results support the continuing collection of multiplex sib-pair families to identify autism-susceptibility genes.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2002

Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: a fixation time study.

J.N. van der Geest; Chantal Kemner; Marinus N. Verbaten; H. van Engeland

BACKGROUND The abnormal gaze behavior of autistic children toward human faces, as observed in daily-life situations, are investigated in two fixation time studies. It has been argued that faces are a special kind of stimuli for normal individuals and that this might not be the case for autistic children. METHODS A group of high-functioning autistic children (including a group of sub-threshold PDD-NOS children) was compared with a group of normal children, with respect to their fixation behavior for photographs of human faces. Using an infrared eye-tracking device, fixation times for the whole face and for the facial elements of faces were compared between the two groups. The first study dealt with faces having an emotional expression. The second study dealt with neutral faces presented either upright or upside-down. RESULTS Results of the two studies showed that autistic children have the same fixation behavior as normal children for upright faces, with or without an emotional expression. Furthermore, results of the second study showed that normal children spent less time looking at upside-down faces, but that the fixation times of autistic children were not influenced by the orientation of the faces. CONCLUSIONS These results plead against the notion that the abnormal gaze behavior in everyday life is due to the presence of facial stimuli per se. Furthermore, the absence of a face orientation effect in autistic children might be a reflection of a lack of holistic processing of human faces in autism.


Biological Psychiatry | 2002

Inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a psychophysiological study of the stop task.

C.C.E. Overtoom; J. Leon Kenemans; Marinus N. Verbaten; Chantal Kemner; Maurits W. van der Molen; Herman van Engeland; Jan K. Buitelaar; Harry S. Koelega

BACKGROUND The purpose of the study was to investigate and identify abnormal brain activity, as revealed by event-related potentials (ERPs) concurring with deficient inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). METHODS Performance and ERPs from 16 children with ADHD and 16 control subjects were compared in the stop-signal paradigm. RESULTS The ADHD children showed a lower inhibition percentage and their (estimated) response time to the stop signal was disproportionally longer compared to the slowing of reaction times to primary-task stimuli. In normal control subjects, fronto-central positivity (100-400 msec) after the onset of the stop-signal was larger in case of successful inhibition, relative to failed inhibition; this was less so in ADHD children. A late positive wave (500-700 msec), maximal at Oz on failed inhibition trials, and possibly related to error-detection, was smaller in ADHD children. CONCLUSIONS These results point to abnormalities in brain processes involved in motor inhibition and error-detection in ADHD children.


Biological Psychiatry | 1995

Auditory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups

Chantal Kemner; Marinus N. Verbaten; Juliane M. Cuperus; Gert Camfferman; Herman van Engeland

ERPs to auditory stimuli, generated during an oddball task, were obtained in a group of autistic children and three control groups (normal, ADDH, and dyslectic children, respectively). The task included the presentation of standards, deviants, and novels and had a (between-group) passive vs. active (counting) condition. It was examined whether 1) it was possible to replicate several earlier findings, 2) autistics manifest an abnormal lateralization pattern of ERPs, 3) autistics have an abnormal mismatch negativity (MMN), and 4) differences between autistics and normals are really specific to the autistic group. The only finding that could be replicated was that autistics have a smaller A/Pcz/300. There was no evidence for abnormal lateralization or abnormal MMN; however, there was an unexpected effect of the task manipulation on the amplitude of the P3: in autistics, the occipital P3 to deviant stimuli was significantly larger in the active than in the passive condition, a finding, like the replication of the smaller A/Pcz/300, specific to the autistic group. It was suggested that the auditory occipital task effect is related to understimulation of the occipital lobe by visual stimuli in autistic children.


Biological Psychiatry | 1997

Event-related potentials and performance of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Children and normal controls in auditory and visual selective attention tasks

Lisa M. Jonkman; Chantal Kemner; Marinus N. Verbaten; Harry S. Koelega; Gert Camfferman; Rutger Jan van der Gaag; Jan K. Buitelaar; Herman van Engeland

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) children and normal controls (7-13 yrs old) performed an auditory and visual selective attention task. Subjects were instructed to respond to the infrequent (10%) stimuli in the relevant channel. Processing negativity (PN) and several other ERP peaks were scored at the midline electrodes. In the auditory task, controls had more correct detections (hits), less false alarms, larger P3b amplitudes to nontarget stimuli (but not to hits), a larger central PN and larger early frontal positivity (100-250 ms) to target stimuli than ADHD subjects. In the visual modality, controls had more correct detections, less false alarms, larger P3b amplitudes to nontarget stimuli (but not to hits), and larger frontal P3(1) amplitudes to infrequent than to frequent stimuli. It was hypothesized that in ADHD children in both the auditory and the visual task, there is a deficit in the activation of the P3b process. Incorrect triggering of the P3b process might be caused by disturbances in other aspects of the attention process, preceding the P3b.


Neuropsychopharmacology | 2000

The effects of a sub-anaesthetic dose of ketamine on human selective attention

Bob Oranje; Bnm van Berckel; Chantal Kemner; J.M. van Ree; R.S. Kahn; Marinus N. Verbaten

A growing number of studies demonstrate that antagonists of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors can induce a broad range of psychophysiological anomalies in healthy subjects similar to those observed in schizophrenia. In this study, the effect of a sub-anaesthetic dose of the non-competitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, on human selective attention was explored. It was hypothesized that ketamine would induce in healthy subjects psychophysiological anomalies that are commonly observed in schizophrenic patients, such as reduced P300 amplitude and a reduction of both mismatch negativity (MMN) and processing negativity (PN). In a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled design, healthy male volunteers (n = 18) were challenged with a sub-anaesthetic dose of ketamine (0.3 mg/kg iv) after which they were tested in a selective attention task. In this task, two types of stimuli were evenly presented to the left or right ear: standard tones (80%) and deviant tones (20%) of either 1000 or 1100 Hz. The duration of a stimulus (95 dB) was 50 ms, the interstimulus intervals were randomized between 1750 and 2150 ms. The volunteer was instructed to push a button as quickly as possible after hearing the deviant tone in a specified ear. Ketamine did not alter performance of the subjects: in both the placebo and drug condition their reaction times for and percentages of hits and false alarms did not differ. Ketamine did, however, reduce PN and the P300 amplitude (both in general and to deviant stimuli in particular). However, no drug effect on MMN was found. In addition, ketamine enhanced the N100 amplitude to deviant stimuli. In conclusion, ketamine induces some of the attentional deficits in healthy controls that are observed in schizophrenic patients. Consequently, reduced glutamatergic activity in the brain may be involved in some of the symptoms of schizophrenia.


Psychological Medicine | 2005

Increased gray-matter volume in medication-naive high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder

Saskia J. M. C. Palmen; Hilleke E. Hulshoff Pol; Chantal Kemner; Hugo G. Schnack; Sarah Durston; Bertine E. Lahuis; René S. Kahn; Herman van Engeland

BACKGROUND To establish whether high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have enlarged brains in later childhood, and if so, whether this enlargement is confined to the gray and/or to the white matter and whether it is global or more prominent in specific brain regions. METHOD Brain MRI scans were acquired from 21 medication-naive, high-functioning children with ASD between 7 and 15 years of age and 21 comparison subjects matched for gender, age, IQ, height, weight, handedness, and parental education, but not pubertal status. RESULTS Patients showed a significant increase of 6% in intracranium, total brain, cerebral gray matter, cerebellum, and of more than 40% in lateral and third ventricles compared to controls. The cortical gray-matter volume was evenly affected in all lobes. After correction for brain volume, ventricular volumes remained significantly larger in patients. CONCLUSIONS High-functioning children with ASD showed a global increase in gray-matter, but not white-matter and cerebellar volume, proportional to the increase in brain volume, and a disproportional increase in ventricular volumes, still present after correction for brain volume. Advanced pubertal development in the patients compared to the age-matched controls may have contributed to the findings reported in the present study.


Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry | 1998

Associations Between Event-Related Potentials and Measures of Attention and Inhibition in the Continuous Performance Task in Children With ADHD and Normal Controls

C.C.E. Overtoom; Marinus N. Verbaten; Chantal Kemner; J. Leon Kenemans; Herman van Engeland; Jan K. Buitelaar; Gert Camfferman; Harry S. Koelega

OBJECTIVES First, to differentiate between inattention and impulsivity based on type of errors made in the AX version of the Continuous Performance Task (CPT), and second, to investigate whether differences in performance between children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and normal controls also occur in specific forms of brain activity, namely event-related potentials (ERPs), presumably related to inattention and impulsivity or inhibition. METHOD Sixteen ADHD and 16 normal control children performed the CPT-AX. ERPs were recorded at occipital (Oz), parietal (Pz), central (Cz), and frontal (Fz) leads. RESULTS The ADHD children had a higher CPT-Inattention score and showed smaller parietal positive waves at a latency of approximately 300 msec in reaction to target stimuli, target P3s, likewise indicating less attention. In contrast, they showed neither higher CPT-Impulsivity nor a smaller frontocentral negative wave at about 200 msec (N2); the N2 is generally seen as reflecting inhibition. A subgroup of children with ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (n = 6) had smaller N2 waves than controls, however. CONCLUSIONS The ADHD group studied showed deficits in attention but not in impulsivity (or inhibition).


Behavioural Brain Research | 2003

Effects of methylphenidate, desipramine, and l-dopa on attention and inhibition in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

C.C.E. Overtoom; Marinus N. Verbaten; Chantal Kemner; J.L. Kenemans; H. van Engeland; Jan K. Buitelaar; M.W. van der Molen; J. van der Gugten; Herman G.M. Westenberg; R. A. A. Maes; Harry S. Koelega

The objective of this study was to investigate the effects of methylphenidate (MPH) on attention and inhibition in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and to establish what the relative contributions of the noradrenergic and dopaminergic systems to this effect were. In addition to MPH, two other drugs were administered in order to affect both transmitter systems more selectively, L-dopa (dopamine (DA) agonist) and desipramine (DMI) (noradrenaline (NA) re-uptake inhibitor). Sixteen children with ADHD performed a stop-task, a laboratory task that measures the ability to inhibit an ongoing action, in a double-blind randomized within-subjects design. Each child received an acute clinical dose of MPH, DMI, L-dopa, and placebo; measures of performance and plasma were determined. The results indicated that inhibition performance was improved under DMI but not under MPH or L-dopa. The response-time to the stop-signal was marginally shortened after intake of DMI. MPH decreased omission and choice-errors and caused faster reaction times to the trials without the stop-tone. No effects of L-dopa whatsoever were noted. Prolactin levels were increased and 5-HIAA levels were lowered under DMI relative to placebo. It is suggested that the effects of MPH on attention are due to a combination of noradrenergic and dopaminergic mechanisms. The improved inhibition under DMI could be serotonergically mediated.


Psychophysiology | 1999

Perceptual and response interference in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and the effects of methylphenidate

Lisa M. Jonkman; Chantal Kemner; Marinus N. Verbaten; H. van Engeland; J.L. Kenemans; Gert Camfferman; Jan K. Buitelaar; Harry S. Koelega

Fourteen children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and 14 normal control children were compared with respect to stimulus- and response-related processes. Subjects with ADHD took part in two additional sessions under methylphenidate or placebo. In both experiments, performance and electrophysiological measures such as the P2, N2, and P3 components of event-related potential and electromyogram (EMG) activity were measured during an Eriksen flanker task. In both groups of children, reaction times (RTs) to arrow stimuli incongruent with the target were longer than those to neutral stimuli (response interference), which were again slower than RTs to target-alone stimuli (perceptual interference). Children with ADHD made more errors to incongruent stimuli and showed more response interference. For correct responses, no differences between the groups in response interference effects on reaction time, P2, N2, and P3 latency, or EMG onset were found. Methylphenidate had a general enhancing effect on accuracy but did not specifically reduce interference from the flanking stimuli. Methylphenidate had no effects on RT, N2 and P2 latency, P3 amplitude or latency, or EMG activity. The conclusion that methylphenidate did not influence response processes contrasts sharply with findings reported by authors using the Sternberg memory search task.

Collaboration


Dive into the Chantal Kemner's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge