Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Claude Bonnet is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Claude Bonnet.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1996

On the relation between stimulus intensity and processing time: Pieron's law and choice reaction time

Delphine Pins; Claude Bonnet

Piéron (1914, 1920, 1952) demonstrated that simple reaction time (SRT) decays as a hyperbolic function of luminance in detection tasks. However, whether such a relationship holds equally for choice reaction time (CRT) has been questioned (Luce, 1986; Nissen, 1977), at least when the task is not brightness discrimination. In two SRT and three CRT experiments, we investigated the function that relates reaction time (RT) to stimulus intensity for five levels of luminance covering the entire mesopic range. The psychophysical experiments consisted of simple detection, two-alternative forced choice (2 AFC) with spatial uncertainty, 2 AFC with semantic categorization, and 2 AFC with orientation discrimination. The results of the experiments showed that mean RT increases with task complexity. However, the exponents of the functions relating RT to stimulus intensity were found to be similar in the different experiments. This finding indicates that Piéron’s law holds for CRT as well as for SRT. It describes RT as a power function of stimulus intensity, with similar exponents, regardless of the complexity of the task.


Vision Research | 1993

Perceived speed of moving lines depends on orientation, length, speed and luminance

Eric Castet; Jean Lorenceau; Maggie Shiffrar; Claude Bonnet

In this study, the perceived speed of a tilted line translating horizontally (for a duration of 167 msec) is evaluated with respect to a vertical line undergoing the same translation. Perceived speed of the oblique line is shown to be underestimated when compared to the vertical line. This bias increases: (1) when the line is further tilted, (2) with greater line lengths, (3) with lower contrasts, and finally (4) with a speed of 2.1 deg/sec as compared to a higher speed of 4.2 deg/sec. These results may be accounted for by considering that two velocity signals are used by the visual system to estimate the speed of the line: the translation of this line (this signal does not depend on the lines orientation) and the motion component normal to the line (this signal depends on orientation). We suggest that these two signals are encoded by different types of units and that the translation signal is specifically extracted at the line endings. We further suggest that these signals are integrated by a weighted average process according to their perceptual salience. Other interpretations are considered at the light of current models dealing with the two-dimensional integration of different velocity signals.


Perception | 1977

Visual Motion Detection Models: Features and Frequency Filters

Claude Bonnet

Two kinds of models have been proposed for taking into account the sensory processes at work in the detection of visual motion: the feature model and the frequency-filter model. The problem of the complementarity of these models is raised. On the basis of empirical data, it is proposed that they are consistent.


Vision Research | 1995

Subthreshold summation with illusory contours

Birgitta Dresp; Claude Bonnet

Results from three experiments using spatial forced-choice techniques show that an illusory contour improves the detectability of a spatially superimposed, thin subthreshold line of either contrast polarity. Furthermore, the subthreshold line is found to enhance the visibility of the illusory contour. Stimuli which do not induce illusory contours, but reduce uncertainty about the spatial position of the line, give rise to a slight detection facilitation, but the threshold of 75% correct responses is not attained. The data indicate that superimposing illusory contours and subthreshold lines produces interactions which are similar to classic subthreshold summation. They thus provide psychophysical evidence for the functional equivalence of illusory contours and real lines suggested by recent neurophysiological findings.


Vision Research | 1991

Psychophysical evidence for low-level processing of illusory contours and surfaces in the kanizsa square

Birgitta Dresp; Claude Bonnet

Increment thresholds were measured on either side of one of the illusory contours of a white-on-black Kanizsa square and on the illusory contour itself. The data show that thresholds are elevated when measured on either side of the illusory border. These elevations diminish with increasing distance of the target spot from the white elements which induce the illusory figure. The most striking result, however, is that threshold elevations are considerably lower or even absent when the target is located on the illusory contour itself. At an equivalent position in a control figure where no illusory contour is visible, such a threshold decrease does not occur. The present observations add empirical support to low-level explanations of illusory contour perception.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1993

Properties of curvilinear vection

Xavier M. Sauvan; Claude Bonnet

Approximately linear relationships were observed between contrast, spatial frequency, temporal frequency, or velocity of stimulation and perceived velocity of curvilinear vection—that is, a visually induced self-motion in a curved path. Similarly, linear relationships were also found between the perceived degree of curvature of curvilinear vection and spatial frequency or velocity of stimulation. Since the perceived velocity of curvilinear vection varies with contrast, spatial frequency, temporal frequency, and angular velocity, and the perceived degree of curvature of curvilinear vection varies only with spatial frequency and angular velocity, peripheral vision is not sufficient for computing accurately the curvilinear component of induced self-motion in a curved path. Concurrently, it was shown that the perceived direction of curvilinear vection is not always unambiguously perceived (Sauvan & Bonnet, 1989). Consequently, it is suggested that two different types of visual processing, which involve the peripheral or the central vision, underlie the processing of curvilinear vection.


Vision Research | 2002

On the delay in processing high spatial frequency visual information: reaction time and VEP latency study of the effect of local intensity of stimulation.

Angel Vassilev; Milena Mihaylova; Claude Bonnet

Saleh and Bonnet [Fechner Day 98, p. 344] have shown that, upon parafoveal stimulation and up to 6.5 c/deg, reaction time (RT) is a function of grating contrast multiplied by grating period. The present experiments extend these findings to foveal stimulation within a wider spatial-frequency (SF) range and to stimuli of different duration. Both RT and latency of visually evoked potentials (VEP) were measured. The findings might be explained by the following assumption: Most RT and VEP latency variations across the SF range are a result of local intensity factors (retinal contrast and width of grating bars). Residual RT variations were found that might be due to processing of high SFs by slower mechanisms than those processing low and medium SFs.


Vision Research | 1993

Psychophysical measures of illusory form perception: Further evidence for local mechanisms

Birgitta Dresp; Claude Bonnet

Detection thresholds for a small light spot were measured at various distances from configurations (Kanizsa squares and other) consisting of white inducing elements on a dark background. Threshold distributions as a function of target position, number, size and spacing of contrast inducing elements were established. The data show that thresholds are elevated when the target is located close to one or more inducing element(s). Furthermore, threshold elevations diminish with increasing distance of the target from the configurations, increasing spacing and decreasing size of their inducing elements. When the target is flashed upon an illusory contour, no threshold elevation is observed in any of the conditions tested. Within incomplete illusory figures (only half of the square visible), the threshold gradients show the same tendencies. The present observations add further empirical support to the idea that illusory figures are built up by way of local mechanisms at early stages of processing.


Perception | 1990

Apparent brightness enhancement in the Kanizsa square with and without illusory contour formation

Birgitta Dresp; Jean Lorenceau; Claude Bonnet

The perceived strength of darkness enhancement in the centre of surfaces surrounded or not surrounded by illusory contours was investigated as a function of proximity of the constituent elements of the display and their angular size. Magnitude estimation was used to measure the perception of the darkness phenomenon in white-on-grey stimuli. Darkness enhancement was perceived in both types of the stimuli used, but more strongly in the presence of illusory contours. In both cases, perceived darkness enhancement increased with increasing proximity of the constituent parts of the display and with their angular size. These results suggest that the occurrence of darkness (or brightness) enhancement phenomena in the centre of the displays is not directly related to illusory contour formation.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1972

Movement detection thresholds and stimulus duration

Ronald Cohen; Claude Bonnet

Movement detection thresholds were measured for varying exposures of a moving spot. A tradeoff was found in which an increase in duration (T) was offset by a decrease in the velocity required for detection (V). In the range of durations studied (about 50–700 msec), V × T was constant. The V × T constancy was interpreted in terms of the direct detection of movement as motion, and a comparison was made with Bloch’s law.

Collaboration


Dive into the Claude Bonnet's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Birgitta Dresp

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jean Lorenceau

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Luciano Mecacci

Sapienza University of Rome

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Carlos Paulos

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Christelle Nithart

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Eric Castet

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jean-Didier Bagot

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge