Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Daniel C. Molden is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Daniel C. Molden.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2003

Transfer of value from fit.

E. Tory Higgins; Lorraine Chen Idson; Antonio L. Freitas; Scott Spiegel; Daniel C. Molden

People experience regulatory fit when they pursue a goal in a manner that sustains their regulatory orientation (E. T. Higgins, 2000). Five studies tested whether the value experienced from regulatory fit can transfer to a subsequent evaluation of an object. In Studies 1 and 2, participants gave the same coffee mug a higher price if they had chosen it with a strategy that fit their orientation (eager strategy/promotion; vigilant strategy/prevention) than a strategy that did not fit. Studies 3-5 investigated possible mechanisms underlying this effect. Value transfer was independent of positive mood, perceived effectiveness (instrumentality), and perceived efficiency (ease), and occurred for an object that w as independent of the fit process itself. The findings supported a value confusion account of transfer.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2001

Promotion and prevention focus on alternative hypotheses: Implications for attributional functions.

Nira Liberman; Daniel C. Molden; Lorraine Chen Idson; E. Tory Higgins

Five studies examined hypothesis generation and discounting in causal attribution from the perspective of regulatory focus theory (E. T. Higgins, 1997, 1998). According to this theory, a promotion focus is associated with generating more and simultaneously endorsing multiple hypotheses, whereas a prevention focus is associated with generating only a few hypotheses and selecting 1 hypothesis from a given set. Five studies confirmed these predictions for both situationally induced and chronic individual differences in regulatory focus. In Studies 1, 2, and 3, individuals in a promotion focus generated more hypotheses than individuals in a prevention focus. In Studies 4 and 5, individuals in a promotion focus discounted explanations in light of alternatives less than individuals in a prevention focus. Study 5 also found that in a promotion focus, person explanations were generalized across situations less than in a prevention focus.


Psychological Science | 2012

Motivational Versus Metabolic Effects of Carbohydrates on Self-Control

Daniel C. Molden; Chin Ming Hui; Abigail A. Scholer; Brian P. Meier; Eric E. Noreen; Paul R. D’Agostino; Valerie Martin

Self-control is critical for achievement and well-being. However, people’s capacity for self-control is limited and becomes depleted through use. One prominent explanation for this depletion posits that self-control consumes energy through carbohydrate metabolization, which further suggests that ingesting carbohydrates improves self-control. Some evidence has supported this energy model, but because of its broad implications for efforts to improve self-control, we reevaluated the role of carbohydrates in self-control processes. In four experiments, we found that (a) exerting self-control did not increase carbohydrate metabolization, as assessed with highly precise measurements of blood glucose levels under carefully standardized conditions; (b) rinsing one’s mouth with, but not ingesting, carbohydrate solutions immediately bolstered self-control; and (c) carbohydrate rinsing did not increase blood glucose. These findings challenge metabolic explanations for the role of carbohydrates in self-control depletion; we therefore propose an alternative motivational model for these and other previously observed effects of carbohydrates on self-control.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2009

Motivations for prevention or promotion following social exclusion: being rejected versus being ignored.

Daniel C. Molden; Gale M. Lucas; Wendi L. Gardner; Kristy K. Dean; Megan L. Knowles

Social exclusion evokes powerful motivations and emotions. The present studies examined how these motivations and emotions might differ following exclusion that is explicit, active, and direct (i.e., when one is rejected) versus implicit, passive, and indirect (i.e., when one is ignored). It was hypothesized that being rejected should produce a sense of social loss and lead to more prevention-focused responses, including withdrawal from social contact, thoughts about actions one should not have taken, and increased feelings of agitation. In contrast, being ignored should produce a sense of failure to achieve social gain and lead to more promotion-focused responses, including reengagement in social contact, thoughts about actions one should have taken, and increased feelings of dejection. These hypotheses were supported across 4 studies in which people recalled or underwent experiences of being rejected or ignored. Past research on active versus passive exclusion is reexamined and found to be consistent with these hypotheses as well.


Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation#R##N#The Search for Optimal Motivation and Performance | 2000

Meaning and motivation

Daniel C. Molden; Carol S. Dweck

Publisher Summary The different meanings given to achievement situations by entity theorists versus incremental theorists are related to, yet distinct from, several constructs that have received attention in the past. Entity theorists believe in fixed intelligence and tend to see achievement tasks as having the power to measure this fixed intelligence. In contrast, incremental theorists believe in malleable, acquirable intelligence and see achievement tasks as either vehicles for increasing intellectual skills or as vehicles for assessing their present, task-specific skills. This chapter examines how the meaning that people assign to an achievement situation affects their motivation—how it affects the goals they pursue, the effectiveness with which they pursue them, and the interest and enjoyment that accompanies their pursuit. It begins by providing a brief history of the field of achievement motivation from a “meaning” perspective. It illustrates how this critical idea emerged but is now in danger of disappearing from current conceptions. Following this, the chapter describes how different people may imbue even the “same” goal with widely different meanings, resulting in widely different motivational patterns. Finally, it explains how this formulation integrates a number of prominent theories within the study of achievement motivation—namely, those theories involving goals, attributions, approach/avoidance motivation, and self-worth.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2010

Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension.

David B. Miele; Daniel C. Molden

Previous research overwhelmingly suggests that feelings of ease people experience while processing information lead them to infer that their comprehension is high, whereas feelings of difficulty lead them to infer that their comprehension is low. However, the inferences people draw from their experiences of processing fluency should also vary in accordance with their naive theories about why new information might be easy or difficult to process. Five experiments that involved reading novel texts showed that participants who view intelligence as a fixed attribute, and who tend to interpret experiences of processing difficulty as an indication that they are reaching the limits of their ability, reported lower levels of comprehension as fluency decreased. In contrast, participants who view intelligence as a malleable attribute that develops through effort, and who do not tend to interpret experiences of processing difficulty as pertaining to some innate ability, did not report lower levels of comprehension as fluency decreased. In fact, when these participants were particularly likely to view effort as leading to increased mastery, decreases in fluency led them to report higher levels of comprehension.


Psychological Science | 2011

Does Easily Learned Mean Easily Remembered? It Depends on Your Beliefs About Intelligence

David B. Miele; Bridgid Finn; Daniel C. Molden

Because numerous studies have shown that feelings of encoding fluency are positively correlated with judgments of learning, a single dominant heuristic, easily learned = easily remembered (ELER), has been posited to explain how people interpret encoding fluency when assessing their own memory. However, the inferences people draw from feelings of encoding fluency may vary with their beliefs about why information is easy or effortful to encode. We conducted two experiments in which participants studied word lists and then predicted their future recall of those items. Results revealed that subjects who viewed intelligence as fixed, and who tended to interpret effortful encoding as indicating that they had reached the limits of their ability, used the ELER heuristic to make judgments of learning. However, subjects who viewed intelligence as malleable, and who tended to interpret effortful encoding as indicating greater engagement in learning, did not use the ELER heuristic and at times predicted greater memory for items that they found more effortful to learn.


Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 2010

There’s No Substitute for Belonging: Self-Affirmation Following Social and Nonsocial Threats

Megan L. Knowles; Gale M. Lucas; Daniel C. Molden; Wendi L. Gardner; Kristy K. Dean

Feelings of belonging are closely linked to feelings of self-esteem. This article examines whether these feelings are regulated in a similar manner. Research on self-esteem maintenance shows that self-enhancement strategies are interchangeable; self-esteem threats in one domain instigate indirect self-affirmations in unrelated domains that effectively replace needs to directly address the original threats. From this perspective, when self-esteem threats arise from a lack of belonging, indirect self-affirmations should again be both preferred and effective. However, belonging regulation may be distinct from self-esteem regulation. From this belonging maintenance perspective, indirect affirmations that enhance esteem, but do not directly repair belonging, may be relatively less preferred and effective following belonging threats. Supporting the belonging maintenance perspective, four studies demonstrated that whereas intelligence threats tended to elicit indirect self-affirmations, belonging threats elicited relatively more direct self-affirmations. Furthermore, whereas indirect affirmation strategies effectively repaired intelligence threats they did not effectively repair belonging threats.


Psychological Science | 2009

Perceived Support for Promotion-Focused and Prevention-Focused Goals Associations With Well-Being in Unmarried and Married Couples

Daniel C. Molden; Gale M. Lucas; Eli J. Finkel; Madoka Kumashiro; Caryl E. Rusbult

Perceived emotional support from close relationship partners in times of stress is a major predictor of well-being. However, recent research has suggested that, beyond emotional support, perceived support for achieving personal goals is also important for well-being. The present study extends such research by demonstrating that associations of perceived goal support with well-being differ depending on how people represent their goals and the general motivational context in which they pursue these goals. Among unmarried romantic partners, for whom the context of the relationship presumably is largely attainment oriented, perceived support for attainment-relevant (or promotion-focused) goals independently predicted relationship and personal well-being, whereas perceived support for maintenance-relevant (or prevention-focused) goals did not. In contrast, among married partners, for whom the context of the relationship presumably is both attainment and maintenance oriented, perceived support for both promotion-focused and prevention-focused goals independently predicted well-being. We discuss the implications for forecasting and improving well-being among married couples.


Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 2009

Incidental Experiences of Regulatory Fit and the Processing of Persuasive Appeals

Anne M. Koenig; Joseph Cesario; Daniel C. Molden; Spee Kosloff; E. Tory Higgins

This article examines how the subjective experiences of “feeling right” from regulatory fit and of “feeling wrong” from regulatory non-fit influence the way people process persuasive messages. Across three studies, incidental experiences of regulatory fit increased reliance on source expertise and decreased resistance to counterpersuasion, whereas incidental experiences of regulatory non-fit increased reliance on argument strength and increased resistance to counterpersuasion. These results suggest that incidental fit and non-fit experiences can produce, respectively, more superficial or more thorough processing of persuasive messages. The mechanisms underlying these effects, and the conditions under which they should and should not be expected, are discussed.

Collaboration


Dive into the Daniel C. Molden's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge