Daria Dronova
Russian Academy of Sciences
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Daria Dronova.
PLOS ONE | 2015
Marina Butovskaya; O. E. Lazebny; V. A. Vasilyev; Daria Dronova; D. V. Karelin; Audax Mabulla; D. V. Shibalev; Todd K. Shackelford; Bernhard Fink; A. P. Ryskov
The androgen receptor (AR) gene polymorphism in humans is linked to aggression and may also be linked to reproduction. Here we report associations between AR gene polymorphism and aggression and reproduction in two small-scale societies in northern Tanzania (Africa)—the Hadza (monogamous foragers) and the Datoga (polygynous pastoralists). We secured self-reports of aggression and assessed genetic polymorphism of the number of CAG repeats for the AR gene for 210 Hadza men and 229 Datoga men (aged 17–70 years). We conducted structural equation modeling to identify links between AR gene polymorphism, aggression, and number of children born, and included age and ethnicity as covariates. Fewer AR CAG repeats predicted greater aggression, and Datoga men reported more aggression than did Hadza men. In addition, aggression mediated the identified negative relationship between CAG repeats and number of children born.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2017
Agnieszka Sorokowska; Piotr Sorokowski; Peter Hilpert; Katarzyna Cantarero; Tomasz Frackowiak; Khodabakhsh Ahmadi; Ahmad M. Alghraibeh; Richmond Aryeetey; Anna Marta Maria Bertoni; Karim Bettache; Sheyla Blumen; Marta Błażejewska; Tiago Bortolini; Marina Butovskaya; Felipe Nalon Castro; Hakan Cetinkaya; Diana Cunha; Daniel David; Oana A. David; Fahd A. Dileym; Alejandra Domínguez Espinosa; Silvia Donato; Daria Dronova; Seda Dural; Jitka Fialová; Maryanne L. Fisher; Evrim Gulbetekin; Aslıhan Hamamcıoğlu Akkaya; Ivana Hromatko; Raffaella Iafrate
Human spatial behavior has been the focus of hundreds of previous research studies. However, the conclusions and generalizability of previous studies on interpersonal distance preferences were limited by some important methodological and sampling issues. The objective of the present study was to compare preferred interpersonal distances across the world and to overcome the problems observed in previous studies. We present an extensive analysis of interpersonal distances over a large data set (N = 8,943 participants from 42 countries). We attempted to relate the preferred social, personal, and intimate distances observed in each country to a set of individual characteristics of the participants, and some attributes of their cultures. Our study indicates that individual characteristics (age and gender) influence interpersonal space preferences and that some variation in results can be explained by temperature in a given region. We also present objective values of preferred interpersonal distances in different regions, which might be used as a reference data point in future studies.
BMC Genetics | 2016
Polina R. Butovskaya; O. E. Lazebny; Evgeniya M. Sukhodolskaya; Vasily A. Vasiliev; Daria Dronova; Juliya N. Fedenok; Aracelli Rosa; Elena N. Peletskaya; A. P. Ryskov; Marina Butovskaya
BackgroundThe oxytocin (OT) system is known to be implicated in the regulation of complex social behavior, particularly empathy and parenting. The goal of this study was to estimate the gender and population differences in polymorphisms of two oxytocin receptor gene SNPs, rs53576 and rs2254298, in four populations.ResultsThese data were compared with each other and with 14 samples from the corresponding regions retrieved from the 1000 Genomes database. Low level of heterozygosity was observed for both SNPs in all populations in this study (rs53576: Catalonian, Hobs = 0.413; Hadza, Hobs = 0.556; sr2254698: Khanty-Mansi, Hobs = 0.250; Datoga, Hobs = 0.550). The amount of variance due to regional variability was almost equal for both SNPs (rs53576: FRT = 0.086, rs2554298: FRT = 0.072), whereas variance for the population level of variability was twice bigger for rs2554298 (rs53576: FST = 0.127, rs2554298: FST = 0.162). Pairwise coefficients of fixation demonstrate that the Hadza were well differentiated from other African populations except of Datoga, the Datoga were weakly differentiated from other African origin populations, the Ob Ugric people were extremely differentiated from all other populations. Catalans were extremely differentiated of Asian populations.ConclusionsIt is hypothesized on the base of spatial distribution of the evolutionary novel A alleles of the both OXTR gene loci, that the spread of alleles of rs22542298 and rs53376 SNPs may be associated to some extant with manipulation of parental investment in humans.
Russian Journal of Genetics | 2014
V. A. Vasilyev; Evgeniya M. Sukhodolskaya; P. V. Kulidzhanov; A. M. Kulikov; O. E. Lazebny; Daria Dronova; Marina Butovskaya; D. V. Shibalev; A. P. Ryskov
A molecular-genetic study of 5-HTTLPR and the Stin2 loci of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTL) in males of the African ethnic populations Hadza and Datoga, which differ in the level of culturally acceptable aggression, was carried out. The distribution of allele and genotype frequencies of these two loci was established. It was shown that the frequency distribution of genotypes and alleles among Hadza and Datoga in the examined samples is practically identical by the VNTR-polymorphism of both loci. However, the Hadza populations, as compared to Datoga, showed a significant (p = 0.006) increase in the frequency of the transcriptionally less active allele LG of the 5-HTTLPR locus. For the first time, the structure of the allelic variant of locus Stin2 with eight repetitions (Stin2.8) is described and established for African populations. The test for independence of the frequency distribution of the alleles of the studied loci showed highly significant linkage disequilibrium among Hadza (p ≪ 0.001) and Datoga (p = 0.021). In analysis of the genotype combinations of two loci, it was revealed that the studied populations differed significantly by the LA/LG 10/12 genotype (p ≪ 0.001). When combining the genotypes, no significant differences between the populations based on their expression activity were identified. We assume that the identified combined genotypes reflect the effects of similar behavioral traits for both populations.
Frontiers in Psychology | 2017
Piotr Sorokowski; Ashley K. Randall; Agata Groyecka; Tomasz Frackowiak; Katarzyna Cantarero; Peter Hilpert; Khodabakhsh Ahmadi; Ahmad M. Alghraibeh; Richmond Aryeetey; Anna Marta Maria Bertoni; Karim Bettache; Marta Błażejewska; Guy Bodenmann; Tiago Bortolini; Carla Bosc; Marina Butovskaya; Felipe Nalon Castro; Hakan Cetinkaya; Diana Cunha; Daniel David; Oana A. David; Alejandra Domínguez Espinosa; Silvia Donato; Daria Dronova; Seda Dural; Maryanne L. Fisher; Aslıhan Hamamcıoğlu Akkaya; Takeshi Hamamura; Karolina Hansen; Wallisen T. Hattori
Forms of committed relationships, including formal marriage arrangements between men and women, exist in almost every culture (Bell, 1997). Yet, similarly to many other psychological constructs (Henrich et al., 2010), marital satisfaction and its correlates have been investigated almost exclusively in Western countries (e.g., Bradbury et al., 2000). Meanwhile, marital relationships are heavily guided by culturally determined norms, customs, and expectations (for review see Berscheid, 1995; Fiske et al., 1998). While we acknowledge the differences existing both between- and within-cultures, we measured marital satisfaction and several factors that might potentially correlate with it based on self-report data from individuals across 33 countries. The purpose of this paper is to introduce the raw data available for anybody interested in further examining any relations between them and other country-level scores obtained elsewhere. Below, we review the central variables that are likely to be related to marital satisfaction. Gender Gender has long been identified in the literature as a predictor of marital satisfaction (Bernard, 1972). Specifically, early works suggested that men report being more satisfied with their marriages compared to women in both Western (e.g., Schumm et al., 1998) and non-Western (e.g., Rostami et al., 2014) cultures. However, sex differences in marital satisfaction may differ across cultures due to traditional sex roles (Pardo et al., 2012) and larger-scale cultural variables, such as sex egalitarianism (Taniguchi and Kaufman, 2013).
Chemical Senses | 2018
Agnieszka Sorokowska; Agata Groyecka; Maciej Karwowski; Tomasz Frackowiak; Jennifer E. Lansford; Khodabakhsh Ahmadi; Ahmad M. Alghraibeh; Richmond Aryeetey; Anna Marta Maria Bertoni; Karim Bettache; Sheyla Blumen; Marta Błażejewska; Tiago Bortolini; Marina Butovskaya; Katarzyna Cantarero; Felipe Nalon Castro; Hakan Cetinkaya; Lei Chang; Bin-Bin Chen; Diana Cunha; Daniel David; Oana A. David; Fahd A. Dileym; Alejandra Domínguez Espinosa; Silvia Donato; Daria Dronova; Seda Dural; Jitka Fialová; Maryanne L. Fisher; Evrim Gulbetekin
Olfaction plays an important role in human social communication, including multiple domains in which people often rely on their sense of smell in the social context. The importance of the sense of smell and its role can however vary inter-individually and culturally. Despite the growing body of literature on differences in olfactory performance or hedonic preferences across the globe, the aspects of a given culture as well as culturally universal individual differences affecting odor awareness in human social life remain unknown. Here, we conducted a large-scale analysis of data collected from 10 794 participants from 52 study sites from 44 countries all over the world. The aim of our research was to explore the potential individual and country-level correlates of odor awareness in the social context. The results show that the individual characteristics were more strongly related than country-level factors to self-reported odor awareness in different social contexts. A model including individual-level predictors (gender, age, material situation, education, and preferred social distance) provided a relatively good fit to the data, but adding country-level predictors (Human Development Index, population density, and average temperature) did not improve model parameters. Although there were some cross-cultural differences in social odor awareness, the main differentiating role was played by the individual differences. This suggests that people living in different cultures and different climate conditions may still share some similar patterns of odor awareness if they share other individual-level characteristics.
Frontiers in Psychology | 2017
Piotr Sorokowski; Ashley K. Randall; Agata Groyecka; Tomasz Frackowiak; Katarzyna Cantarero; Peter Hilpert; Khodabakhsh Ahmadi; Ahmad M. Alghraibeh; Richmond Aryeetey; Anna Marta Maria Bertoni; Karim Bettache; Marta Błażejewska; Guy Bodenmann; Tiago Bortolini; Carla Bosc; Marina Butovskaya; Felipe Nalon Castro; Hakan Cetinkaya; Diana Cunha; Daniel David; Oana A. David; Fahd A. Dileym; Alejandra Domínguez Espinosa; Silvia Donato; Daria Dronova; Seda Dural; Maryanne L. Fisher; Aslıhan Hamamcıoğlu Akkaya; Takeshi Hamamura; Karolina Hansen
[This corrects the article on p. 1199 in vol. 8, PMID: 28785230.].
Frontiers in Psychology | 2016
Peter Hilpert; Ashley K. Randall; Piotr Sorokowski; David C. Atkins; Agnieszka Sorokowska; Khodabakhsh Ahmadi; Ahmad M. Alghraibeh; Richmond Aryeetey; Anna Marta Maria Bertoni; Karim Bettache; Marta Błażejewska; Guy Bodenmann; Jessica Borders; Tiago Bortolini; Marina Butovskaya; Felipe Nalon Castro; Hakan Cetinkaya; Diana Cunha; Oana A. David; Anita DeLongis; Fahd A. Dileym; Alejandra Domínguez Espinosa; Silvia Donato; Daria Dronova; Seda Dural; Maryanne L. Fisher; Tomasz Frackowiak; Evrim Gulbetekin; Aslıhan Hamamcıoğlu Akkaya; Karolina Hansen
Archive | 2016
Polina R. Butovskaya; O. E. Lazebny; Evgeniya M. Sukhodolskaya; Vasily A. Vasiliev; Daria Dronova; Juliya N. Fedenok; Aracelli Rosa; Elena N. Peletskaya; A. P. Ryskov; Marina Butovskaya
Frontiers in Psychology | 2016
Peter Hilpert; Ashley K. Randall; Piotr Sorokowski; David C. Atkins; Agnieszka Sorokowska; Khodabakhsh Ahmadi; Ahmad M. Alghraibeh; Richmond Aryeetey; Anna Marta Maria Bertoni; Karim Bettache; Marta Błażejewska; Guy Bodenmann; Jessica Borders; Tiago Bortolini; Marina Butovskaya; Felipe Nalon Castro; Hakan Cetinkaya; Diana Cunha; Oana A. David; Anita DeLongis; Fahd A. Dileym; Alejandra Domínguez Espinosa; Silvia Donato; Daria Dronova; Seda Dural; Maryanne L. Fisher; Tomasz Frackowiak; Evrim Gulbetekin; Aslıhan Hamamcıoğlu Akkaya; Karolina Hansen