David B. Peele
Research Triangle Park
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by David B. Peele.
Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology | 1991
David B. Peele; S.D. Allison; B. Bolon; J.D. Prah; K.F. Jensen; Kevin T. Morgan
Methods for assessing functional consequences of olfactory mucosal damage were examined in rats exposed to 3-methylindole (3-MI). Treatment with 3-MI (400 mg/kg) induced severe degeneration of olfactory sensory epithelium followed by regeneration, fibrous adhesions, and osseous remodeling of the nasal passages. At 100 mg/kg, there was mild Bowmans gland hypertrophy while the sensory epithelium remained intact. Rats receiving 3-MI demonstrated a treatment-related deficit in acquiring an olfactory learning task which was not due to altered cognitive abilities, as determined by subsequent testing in a step-through passive avoidance task. The results confirm the conclusion that alterations in functional indices resulted from 3-MI-induced anosmia and demonstrate the utility of simple learning tasks in assessing functional capacity following olfactory epithelial damage in rats.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior | 1988
David B. Peele; Scott P. Baron
The effects of post-selection confinement (delays) on both the acquisition of performance and the response to the muscarinic blocker, scopolamine, were examined in an automated version of the eight arm radial maze. Long-Evans rats, exposed to post-selection delays of 0.5 sec (n = 4) or 100 sec (n = 4) during daily training trials did not differ in either the number of trials to acquire an accurate baseline of performance or in the amount of time required to obtain all eight food pellets. However, the pattern (delta-arm scores) of within-session arm selections demonstrated by the two groups of rats differed. Rats exposed to the 0.5-sec delay typically selected arms adjacent to arms from which they exited while rats exposed to the 100-sec delay were more likely to enter arms 2-removed from the exit arm. When scopolamine (0.03 to 1.0 mg/kg) was administered prior to testing, rats in the 100-sec delay group showed a greater reduction of accuracy and a larger increase in selection latency than rats in the 0.5-sec delay group. The differential effect of delay value on delta-arm scores was also eliminated in a dosage dependent manner with scopolamine. Scopolamine methylbromide (0.3 mg/kg) was found to have little effect on performance. In summary, the results indicate that the post-selection delay procedure is a sensitive and selective test for chemical-induced dysfunctioning of spatial memory in rats.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews | 1989
David B. Peele; Antonia Vincent
Tests of learning and memory are currently not typically included in first-tier screening batteries even though there is ample evidence that chemical exposure can produce deficits in these cognitive processes. The approach taken in behavioral toxicology has been to restrict these cognitive tests to second tier or hazard characterization studies, yet there is little agreement on which tests are most appropriate. The present survey was designed to determine the current testing strategies in toxicology for detecting and characterizing the effects of chemical treatment on learning and memory, and to make comparisons to similar data from the fields of psychopharmacology and neurobiology. The survey data revealed a number of discipline-dependent effects on the selection of tests. A number of these effects were clearly related to the subject matter as well as the particular chemical/treatment being examined. Given the youth of the field, behavioral toxicology has the advantage of gaining valuable information from both of these disciplines. Behavioral testing in neurotoxicology should consider strategies which maximize unification of these closely related fields of neuroscience.
Neurotoxicology and Teratology | 1987
David B. Peele; Kevin M. Crofton
Pyrethroid insecticides have been divided into Types I and II based on behavioral profiles of toxicity produced by life-threatening dosages. In order to assess potential alterations in acquired (operant) behavior, acute dosage-effect and time-course determinations for permethrin (Type I) and cypermethrin (Type II) were made. Long-Evans rats responded for food according to a multiple schedule consisting of four different variable-interval schedules. Permethrin (100-400 mg/kg) and cypermethrin (7.5-60 mg/kg) were administered PO 1.5 hr pre-session and their effects on response rates and between-component response patterning determined. Permethrin reduced responding in a manner which was independent of the baseline response rate, while the rate reductions following cypermethrin administration showed a dependence on the baseline levels of responding, with low response rates showing differential sensitivity to disruption. When select dosages of each compound were delivered at various pre-session times, onset of and recovery from the rate-decreasing effects were more rapid with cypermethrin, with rates returning to baseline levels by 12 hr post-dosing. Responding was maximally suppressed 24 hr after administration of permethrin and returned to baseline levels 48 hr after administration. The disruption of response patterning following cypermethrin was maximal at 1.5 hr after administration, with complete recovery 12 hr post-dosing. Differential effects on response patterning, in potency, and in the time-course of effects of permethrin and cypermethrin suggest a type-specificity for pyrethroid effects on schedule-controlled behavior at dosages far below those producing lethality in rats.
Psychobiology | 2013
Kevin M. Crofton; Karen F. Dean; Larry P. Sheets; David B. Peele
The experiments reported here were designed to determine the role of associative conditioning in reflex modification of the acoustic startle response, using gaps in background noise. The first experiment was done to characterize the effects of repeated testing for 9 days with 20-msec gaps in white noise as the preliminary stimulus (SI) and a 120-dB, 40-msec 13-kHz tone as the eliciting stimulus (S2). The second experiment was a test of associative conditioning. Three groups of rats were tested daily for 6 days under one of the following conditions: SI and S2 paired in a contingent manner, S2 only, or SI only. All groups then received the contingent pairing of SI and S2 for an additional 9 days of testing. In the third experiment, a separate group of rats was tested with either contingent or noncontingent presentation of the SI and S2 for 6 days, and then, on the 7th day, both groups received the stimuli in a contingent fashion. Results indicate that the amount of inhibition increases with repeated, daily testing, and that it achieves asymptotic levels of inhibition after 5-6 daily sessions. The presentation of SI and S2 together is a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for normal development of inhibition. Alternatively, the contingency that exists between the two stimuli is likely to be the determining factor necessary for development of the inhibition. These data imply that an associative learning process may be a major factor in the gap-inhibition phenomenon.
Toxicological Sciences | 1995
Mark E. Stanton; Kevin M. Crofton; L. Earl Gray; Christopher J. Gordon; Williams K. Boyes; M. Leonard Mole; David B. Peele; Philip J. Bushnell
The prospect of widespread human exposure associated with its use as an alternative fuel has sparked concern about the toxic potential of inhaled methanol (MeOH). Previous studies have revealed congenital malformations in rats following inhaled MeOH (Nelson et al. (1985). Fundam. Appl. Toxicol. 5, 727-736) but these studies did not include postnatal behavioral assessment. In the present study, pregnant Long-Evans rats were placed in exposure chambers containing 15,000 ppm MeOH or air for 7 hr/day on Gestational Days (GD) 7-19. The total alveolar dose of methanol was estimated at about 6.1 g/kg/day, for a total dose of about 42.7 g/kg for the entire study. Maternal body weights were recorded daily and blood methanol concentrations were determined at the end of exposure on GD 7, 10, 14, and 18. Following birth (Postnatal Day 0 [PND 0]), a number of tests were performed at various points in development, including: offspring mortality and body wt (PND 1,3), motor activity (PND 13-21, 30, 60), olfactory learning (PND 18), behavioral thermoregulation (PND 20-21), T-maze learning (PND 23-24), acoustic startle response (PND 24, 60), reflex modification audiometry (PND 60), pubertal landmarks (PND 31-56), passive avoidance (PND 72), and visual-evoked potentials (PND 160). Maternal blood MeOH levels, measured from samples taken within 15 min after removal from the exposure chamber, declined from about 3.8 mg/ml on the first day of exposure to 3.1 mg/ml on the 12th day of exposure. MeOH transiently reduced maternal body wt (4-7%) on GD 8-10, and offspring BW (5%) on PND 1. No other test revealed significant effects of MeOH. Prenatal exposure to high levels of inhaled MeOH appears to have little effect on this broad battery of tests beyond PND 1 in the rat.
Neurotoxicology and Teratology | 1994
Jordi Llorens; Kevin M. Crofton; David B. Peele
Abstract 3,3′-Iminodipropionitrile (IDPN) has been reported to disrupt learning and memory in rats (24). The present work addressed the effects of IDPN on tasks requiring the use of spatial information. Separate groups of male rats were dosed with IDPN (IP, in 1 ml/kg saline) for 3 consecutive days and tested in the following procedures: (a) step-through passive avoidance conditioning (0, 100, 150, and 200 mg/kg/day); (b) Morris water maze (MWM) acquisition and retention (0, 125, 150, 175, and 200 mg/kg/day); (c) radial arm maze (RAM) acquisition (0, 100, 200, and 400 mg/kg/day); (d) RAM steady-state performance (0, 200, and 400 mg/kg/day); (e) repeated acquisition in the RAM (0, and 200 mg/kg/day). The vestibular toxicity of IDPN resulted in alterations in spontaneous behavior or swimming deficits in 5 of 8 rats treated with 175 mg/kg/day and in all the animals dosed with 200 or 400 mg/kg/day. IDPN increased step-through PA latencies at 200 mg/kg/day but not at lower doses. In the MWM, no performance deficits were observed at the dose levels preserving the swimming ability of the animals. In both the acquisition and the steady-state RAM tasks, IDPN (400 mg/kg/day) induced an increase in both choice errors and perseverative errors. In the RAM repeated acquisition paradigm, IDPN (200 mg/kg/day) induced performance deficits that included a decreased rate of within-session reduction in errors. The present data show that IDPN disrupts performance of tasks requiring spatial learning and memory and indicate that these deficits can be in part caused by an acquisition deficit.
Psychopharmacology | 1989
David B. Peele; Jackie D. Farmer; Janice E. Coleman
Trimethylin (TMT) produces behavioral and cognitive deficits resulting, in part, from limbic system toxicity. To determine whether these effects result from learning deficits or accelerated memory loss, the present experiment examined two delay conditioning paradigms in rats previously treated with either saline or TMT. Saline-treated Long-Evans rats receiving injections of lithium after consuming saccharin-flavored water later avoided saccharin ingestion: the degree of avoidance varied inversely with the time (0.5, 3 or 6 h) separating initial saccharin availability and lithium injection. Rats treated with TMT (8 mg/kg IV, 30 days prior) showed impaired conditioning at the long but not the short or intermediate delay conditions, suggesting that the deficits were mnemonic and not associative. Similar delay-dependent deficits in rats treated with TMT were observed in a passive avoidance task that arranged one of two delays between response emission and shock delivery during training. The effects of TMT on delay conditioning were accompanied by reduced bodyweight and hippocampal pathology. In summary, TMT appears to alter the temporally dependent association of events (entering darkened compartment versus saccharin consumption) and consequences (foot shock versus lithium administration) during acquisition. Furthermore, the observed deficits in delay conditioning produced by TMT did not appear to be task specific, with similar effects determined with tests of both somatosensory and gustatory avoidance learning designed to distinguish between functional alterations due to deficits in memorial processes from those due to altered sensory, motor, or associative processes.
International Journal of Toxicology | 1989
David B. Peele
The goal of this article is to outline a strategy for assessing chemical-induced dysfunction of learning and memory in laboratory animals. Toward that end, several questions are raised, including if tests of learning and memory should be included in a primary screening effort, what considerations should guide the selection of particular test methods, and experimental designs. Examples are provided demonstrating that tests of learning and memory can be simple and cost-effective, yet still provide meaningful data on the specificity of effects and on the neural mechanisms involved in chemically induced neurotoxicity.
Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology | 1987
David B. Peele; Jackie D. Farmer; Robert C. MacPhail
A series of studies investigated the conditioned flavor aversions induced by administration of either lead or thallium in combination with either dimercaprol or dimercaptosuccinic acid in an attempt to correlate changes in flavor-aversion conditioning to changes known to alter the toxicity of metal administration. Rats received po administration of either thallium sulfate or lead acetate given alone or in combination with either dimercaprol or dimercaptosuccinic acid after consuming saccharin. Three days later they were given the choice between consuming saccharin or water, and saccharin preferences were recorded. When compared to rats receiving either nothing or the vehicle, rats receiving either lead or thallium showed significant reductions in saccharin preferences (i.e., conditioned flavor aversions). Rats receiving lead acetate in combination with either of the two chelators displayed significantly reduced conditioned flavor aversions when compared to the aversions induced in rats receiving lead alone. Under the same conditions, there were no differences in the conditioned flavor aversions of rats receiving thallium only and those of rats receiving thallium in combination with either of the two chelators. Attenuation of the lead-induced conditioned flavor aversions was eliminated when chelator administration was delayed by 4 hr. This attenuation of lead-induced but not thallium-induced aversions by dimercaprol and dimercaptosuccinic acid demonstrates the sensitivity and selectivity of the flavor-aversion conditioning paradigm in characterizing metal-chelator interactions and is in agreement with clinical reports of effective chelation therapy in cases of lead but not thallium intoxication.