David Caplan
Harvard University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by David Caplan.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences | 1999
David Caplan; Gloria Waters
This target article discusses the verbal working memory system used in sentence comprehension. We review the concept of working memory as a short-duration system in which small amounts of information are simultaneously stored and manipulated in the service of accomplishing a task. We summarize the argument that syntactic processing in sentence comprehension requires such a storage and computational system. We then ask whether the working memory system used in syntactic processing is the same as that used in verbally mediated tasks that involve conscious controlled processing. Evidence is brought to bear from various sources: the relationship between individual differences in working memory and individual differences in the efficiency of syntactic processing; the effect of concurrent verbal memory load on syntactic processing; and syntactic processing in patients with poor short-term memory, patients with poor working memory, and patients with aphasia. Experimental results from these normal subjects and patients with various brain lesions converge on the conclusion that there is a specialization in the verbal working memory system for assigning the syntactic structure of a sentence and using that structure in determining sentence meaning that is separate from the working memory system underlying the use of sentence meaning to accomplish other functions. We present a theory of the divisions of the verbal working memory system and suggestions regarding its neural basis.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 1998
David Caplan; Nathaniel M. Alpert; Gloria Waters
Positron emission tomography (PET) was used to determine regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) as a function of the syntactic form and propositional density of sentences. rCBF increased in the left pars opercularis, part of Brocas area, when subjects processed syntactically more complex sentences. There were no differences in rCBF in the perisylvian association cortex traditionally associated with language processing when subjects made plausibility judgments about sentences with two propositions as compared to sentences with one proposition, but rCBF increased in infero-posterior brain regions. These results suggest that there is a specialization of neural tissue in Brocas area for constructing aspects of the syntactic form of sentences to determine sentence meaning. They also suggest that this specialization is separate from the brain systems that are involved in utilizing the meaning of a sentence that has been understood to accomplish a task.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 1996
Gloria Waters; David Caplan
Ninety-four subjects were tested on the Daneman and Carpenter (1980) reading span task, four versions of a related sentence span task in which reaction times and accuracy on sentence processing were measured along with sentence-final word recall, two number generation tasks designed to test working memory, digit span, and two shape-generation tasks designed to measure visual-spatial working memory. Forty-four subjects were retested on a subset of these measures at a 3-month interval. All subjects were tested on standard vocabulary and reading tests. Correlational analyses showed better internal consistency and test-retest reliability of the sentence span tasks than of the Daneman-Carpenter reading span task. Factor analysis showed no factor that could be related to a central verbal working memory; rotated factors suggested groupings of tests into factors that correspond to digit related tasks, spatial tasks, sentence processing in sentence span tasks, and recall in sentence span tasks. Correlational analyses and regression analyses showed that the sentence processing component of the sentence span tasks was the best predictor of performance on the reading test, with a small independent contribution of the recall component. The results suggest that sentence span tasks are unreliable unless measurements are made of both their sentence processing and recall components, and that the predictive value of these tasks for reading comprehension abilities lies in the overlap of operations rather than in limitations in verbal working memory that apply to both.
Human Brain Mapping | 2000
David Caplan; Nathaniel M. Alpert; Gloria Waters; Anthony Olivieri
Regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was measured with positron emission tomography (PET) when 11 subjects made plausibility judgments about written sentences that varied in their syntactic complexity. While making their judgments, subjects uttered the word “double” aloud at a rate of one utterance per second to inhibit their ability to rehearse the sentences. Blood flow increased in Brocas area when subjects made judgments about the more complex sentences. This result replicates and extends previous findings that blood flow increases in this region when subjects process complex syntax under no interference conditions. The results of this experiment provide strong evidence that the increase in blood flow seen in Brocas area in association with processing syntactically complex structures is not due to subvocal rehearsal of those structures, but rather results from processing syntactic forms themselves. Hum. Brain Mapping 9:65–71, 2000.
Language | 1981
Edward H. Matthel; David Caplan
These fifteen contributions by well-known linguists, psychologists, and neuroscientists explore the new concepts and themes that extend and revise previously held ideas about the biology of cognition.
Neuropsychologia | 1975
John C. Marshall; David Caplan; Jane M. Holmes
Abstract We discuss the issue of measuring ear and visual field advantages; attention is drawn to the problem of constructing measures of laterality which are independent of the fact that subjects differ considerably in the overall accuracy of their performance. Geometrical representations are given for various laterality coefficients; the concept of an isolaterality line then becomes formally analogous to a ROC curve as studied in signal-detection theory. Finally, some suggestions are made concerning the type of experimentation which may allow one to choose between the different laterality coefficients proposed in the literature.
Cognition | 1985
David Caplan; Catherine Baker; Francois Dehaut
Abstract We present the results of three studies of aphasic comprehension of syntactic structure. Group analysis reveals effects of syntactic structure upon correct interpretation of sentences and indicates that separate aspects of syntactic structure contribute additively to sentence complexity. Identifiable subgroups of patients vary in overall ability on this task, and some subgroups show quite isolated impairments with specific sentence types. Subgroups do not correspond to classical aphasia syndromes, and lesion site does not correlate with patient subgroups. The results bear on the nature of impaired syntactic comprehension in aphasia and on aspects of the normal parser/interpreter.
NeuroImage | 1999
David Caplan; Nathaniel M. Alpert; Gloria Waters
Sixteen subjects made plausibility judgments regarding auditorily presented cleft object and cleft subject sentences (It was the actress that the award thrilled; It was the award that thrilled the actress). rCBF increased in Brocas area, pars triangularis, when subjects processed the syntactically more complex cleft object sentences. The results are consistent with previous experiments using written materials and suggest that an increase in rCBF in Brocas area is associated with processing syntactically more complex sentences.
Brain and Language | 1986
David Caplan; Christine Futter
We present the results of a study of sentence comprehension in an agrammatic patient. The patient showed striking regularities in interpreting a wide variety of sentence structures. We analyze her performance as the result of attempting to interpret a simplified syntactic structure, consisting of a linear sequence of lexical categories, according to a number of simple interpretive strategies.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2003
Gina R. Kuperberg; Phillip J. Holcomb; Tatiana Sitnikova; Douglas N. Greve; Anders M. Dale; David Caplan
The aim of this study was to gain further insights into how the brain distinguishes between meaning and syntax during language comprehension. Participants read and made plausibility judgments on sentences that were plausible, morpho-syntactically anomalous, or pragmatically anomalous. In an event-related potential (ERP) experiment, morphosyntactic and pragmatic violations elicited significant P600 and N400 effects, respectively, replicating previous ERP studies that have established qualitative differences in processing conceptually and syntactic anomalies. Our main focus was a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study in which the same subjects read the same sentences presented in the same pseudorandomized sequence while performing the same task as in the ERP experiment. Rapid-presentation event-related fMRI methods allowed us to estimate the hemodynamic response at successive temporal windows as the sentences unfolded word by word, without assumptions about the shape of the underlying response function. Relative to nonviolated sentences, the pragmatic anomalies were associated with an increased hemodynamic response in left temporal and inferior frontal regions and a decreased response in the right medial parietal cortex. Relative to nonviolated sentences, the morphosyntactic anomalies were associated with an increased response in bilateral medial and lateral parietal regions and a decreased response in left temporal and inferior frontal regions. Thus, overlapping neural networks were modulated in opposite directions to the two types of anomaly. These fMRI findings document both qualitative and quantitative differences in how the brain distinguishes between these two types of anomalies. This suggests that morphosyntactic and pragmatic information can be processed in different ways but by the same neural systems.