Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where David I. Anderson is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by David I. Anderson.


Infancy | 2000

Travel broadens the mind.

Joseph J. Campos; David I. Anderson; Marianne Barbu-Roth; Edward M. Hubbard; Matthew J. Hertenstein; David C. Witherington

The onset of locomotion heralds one of the major life transitions in early development and involves a pervasive set of changes in perception, spatial cognition, and social and emotional development. Through a synthesis of published and hitherto unpublished findings, gathered from a number of converging research designs and methods, this article provides a comprehensive review and reanalysis of the consequences of self-produced locomotor experience. Specifically, we focus on the role of locomotor experience in changes in social and emotional development, referential gestural communication, wariness of heights, the perception of self-motion, distance perception, spatial search, and spatial coding strategies. Our analysis reveals new insights into the specific processes by which locomotor experience brings about psychological changes. We elaborate these processes and provide new predictions about previously unsuspected links between locomotor experience and psychological function. The research we describe is relevant to our broad understanding of the developmental process, particularly as it pertains to developmental transitions. Although acknowledging the role of genetically mediated developmental changes, our viewpoint is a transactional one in which a single acquisition, in this case the onset of locomotion, sets in motion a family of experiences and processes that in turn mobilize both broad-based and context-specific psychological reorganizations. We conclude that, in infancy, the onset of locomotor experience brings about widespread consequences, and after infancy, can be responsible for an enduring role in development by maintaining and updating existing skills.


Journal of Sports Sciences | 2002

Sport competition as a dynamical self-organizing system

Tim McGarry; David I. Anderson; Stephen A. Wallace; Mike D. Hughes; Ian M. Franks

The existence of structure in sport competition is implicated in the widespread practice of using the information gathered from a past contest to prepare for a future contest. Based on this reasoning, we previously analysed squash match-play for evidence of signature traits from among the stochastic relations between the various types of shot. The mixed findings from these analyses led us to re-analyse squash match-play as a dynamical system. Here, we extend this line of investigation with some suggestions as to how various sports might be described further within this theoretical framework. We offer some examples of dynamical interactions in dyadic (i.e. one vs one) and team (e.g. many vs many) sports, as well as some predictions from a dynamical systems analysis for these types of sports contests. This paper should serve to initiate further research into the complex interactions that occur in sport competition.


Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport | 1996

The Contextual Interference Effect in Parameter Modifications of the Same Generalized Motor Program

Hiroshi Sekiya; Richard A. Magill; David I. Anderson

This study extended previous work (Sekiya, Magill, Sidaway, & Anderson, 1994) by examining whether the contextual interference (CI) effect could be found when task variations were controlled by the same generalized motor program (GMP) but differentiated on the basis of overall force parameter modifications. A subsidiary aim of this study was to determine how the amount of practice influenced the CI effect. During 2 acquisition sessions and 2 retention sessions, participants (N = 24) performed 3 task variations in either a blocked (low CI) or serial (high CI) condition. The task variations shared the same relative force structures but differed in the amount of overall force that had to be produced. Analysis of a general error measure revealed a strong CI effect, indicating that overall force parameter modifications of the same GMP created the CI effect. Analyses of various dependent measures, which dissociated GMP from parameter components with regard to force characteristics, revealed that parameter learning, but not GMP learning, was enhanced by high CI practice. No CI effect occurred in any aspect of timing characteristics. The amount of practice used in the present study did not influence the efficacy of the CI effect.


Developmental Psychology | 2008

Locomotor Experience Affects Self and Emotion

Ichiro Uchiyama; David I. Anderson; Joseph J. Campos; David C. Witherington; Carl B. Frankel; Laure Lejeune; Marianne Barbu-Roth

Two studies investigated the role of locomotor experience on visual proprioception in 8-month-old infants. Visual proprioception refers to the sense of self-motion induced in a static person by patterns of optic flow. A moving room apparatus permitted displacement of an entire enclosure (except for the floor) or the side walls and ceiling. In Study 1, creeping infants and prelocomotor/walker infants showed significantly greater postural compensation and emotional responses to side wall movement than did same-age prelocomotor infants. Study 2 used true random assignment of prelocomotor infants to locomotor-training (via a powered-mobility device) and no-training conditions. Experimental infants showed powerful effects of locomotor training. These results imply that locomotor experience is playing a causal role in the ontogeny of visual proprioception.


Journal of Motor Behavior | 2005

Support for an explanation of the guidance effect in motor skill learning

David I. Anderson; Richard A. Magill; Hiroshi Sekiya; Greg Ryan

The authors investigated whether the knowledge of results (KR) schedule influences the extent to which intrinsic feedback is noticed and used. Fifty-six participants received KR that was either delayed over 2 trials (Delay-2) or provided directly after each trial (Delay-0) during 160 trials of an unfamiliar aiming task. No-KR retention tests were given after 80 trials and 1 min and 24 hr after the end of acquisition. After retention, all participants were questioned about their use of intrinsic feedback during practice and whether those sources changed as a function of practice. The Delay-2 group performed significantly less accurately on the 1st and last blocks of acquisition trials but showed a significantly smaller performance decline from acquisition to retention. Moreover, the Delay-2 group noticed and used a greater variety of intrinsic feedback sources and its members were more likely to report that their usage changed with practice.


Journal of Motor Behavior | 2001

Motor Learning as a Function of KR Schedule and Characteristics of Task-Intrinsic Feedback

David I. Anderson; Richard A. Magill; Hiroshi Sekiya

Abstract Forty participants (age range = 18–35 years) practiced 1 of 2 versions of an aiming task (with or without spring resistance). Knowledge of results (KR) was provided to them either immediately or after a delay of 2 trials. Immediate KR led to significantly more accurate performance during the 80 trials in acquisition but significantly less accurate performance on a 40-trial retention test given 24 hr after practice. In addition, the spring version of the task was performed significantly less accurately than the no-spring version on the 24-hr retention test. Most important, a significant interaction on the 24-hr retention test revealed that performance of the no-spring version of the task, when KR had been given after a 2-trial delay, was significantly more accurate than performance of the other 3 combinations of task version and KR schedule. The results suggest that KR dependency in motor skill learning is related to familiarity with task-intrinsic feedback in addition to the schedule on which KR is presented.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2013

The role of locomotion in psychological development

David I. Anderson; Joseph J. Campos; David C. Witherington; Audun Dahl; Monica Rivera; Minxuan He; Ichiro Uchiyama; Marianne Barbu-Roth

The psychological revolution that follows the onset of independent locomotion in the latter half of the infants first year provides one of the best illustrations of the intimate connection between action and psychological processes. In this paper, we document some of the dramatic changes in perception-action coupling, spatial cognition, memory, and social and emotional development that follow the acquisition of independent locomotion. We highlight the range of converging research operations that have been used to examine the relation between locomotor experience and psychological development, and we describe recent attempts to uncover the processes that underlie this relation. Finally, we address three important questions about the relation that have received scant attention in the research literature. These questions include: (1) What changes in the brain occur when infants acquire experience with locomotion? (2) What role does locomotion play in the maintenance of psychological function? (3) What implications do motor disabilities have for psychological development? Seeking the answers to these questions can provide rich insights into the relation between action and psychological processes and the general processes that underlie human development.


Human Movement Science | 2012

Kicking performance in relation to balance ability over the support leg

Tracey S-Y Chew-Bullock; David I. Anderson; Kate A. Hamel; Mark Gorelick; Stephen A. Wallace; Ben Sidaway

Balance control is presumed to be a fundamental constraint on the organization of skilled movement. The current experiment explored whether single-leg balance ability predicted kicking performance on the other leg. Thirty-eight participants ranging widely in skill kicked a soccer ball with the right and left legs for maximum accuracy and velocity and performed single-leg balance on a force plate for 30 s with the right and left legs. Significant correlations between single-leg balance and kicking accuracy, but not velocity, were found. Left leg balance was more highly correlated than right leg balance with right (dominant) leg kicking accuracy. However, the same pattern of relations was not seen between single-leg balance and left (non-dominant) leg kicking accuracy. These findings provide preliminary support for the importance of balance ability in kicking performance. The importance of balance in the production of athletic skills is discussed and additional experimental paradigms are suggested that might further our knowledge in this area.


Child Development | 2009

Neonatal Stepping in Relation to Terrestrial Optic Flow.

Marianne Barbu-Roth; David I. Anderson; Adeline Desprès; Joëlle Provasi; D. Cabrol; Joseph J. Campos

This experiment examined whether newborn stepping, a primitive form of bipedal locomotion, could be modulated by optical flow. Forty-eight 3-day-old infants were exposed to optical flows that were projected onto a horizontal surface above which the infants were suspended. Significantly more air steps were elicited by exposure to a terrestrial optical flow specifying forward translation than by a rotating optical flow or a static optical pattern. Thus, a rudimentary coupling between optical flow and stepping is present at birth, suggesting a precocious capacity in the newborn to perceive and utilize visual information specifying self-motion. The findings may help the early diagnosis of infants with visual or visual-motor deficits and the development of visually based interventions for disabled infants.


Consciousness and Cognition | 2012

Does self-efficacy mediate transfer effects in the learning of easy and difficult motor skills?

David J. Stevens; David I. Anderson; Nicholas J. O'Dwyer; A. Mark Williams

The effect of task difficulty on inter-task transfer is a classic issue in motor learning. We examined the relation between self-efficacy and transfer of learning after practicing different versions of a stick balancing task. Practicing the same task or an easier version led to significant pre- to post-test transfer of learning, whereas practicing a more difficult version did not. Self-efficacy increased modestly from pre- to post-test with easy practice, but decreased significantly with difficult practice. In addition, self-efficacy immediately prior to the post-test was significantly lower after difficult practice than easy or intermediate practice. Self-efficacy immediately prior to the post-test, performance at the end of practice, and pre-test performance explained 75% of the variance in post-test performance. The mediating role of self-efficacy on transfer of learning offers an alternative explanation for recent findings on the superiority of easy-to-difficult transfer and may help clarify inconsistencies in earlier research.

Collaboration


Dive into the David I. Anderson's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Stephen A. Wallace

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Audun Dahl

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kate A. Hamel

San Francisco State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Michael Trujillo

San Francisco State University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge