Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where David P. Roeltgen is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by David P. Roeltgen.


American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A | 2008

Cognitive and motor development during childhood in boys with Klinefelter syndrome.

Judith L. Ross; David P. Roeltgen; Gerry A. Stefanatos; Rebecca Benecke; Martha P.D. Zeger; Harvey Kushner; Purita Ramos; Frederick F. Elder; Andrew R. Zinn

The goal of this study was to expand the description of the cognitive development phenotype in boys with Klinefelter syndrome (47,XXY). We tested neuropsychological measures of memory, attention, visual‐spatial abilities, visual‐motor skills, and language. We examined the influence of age, handedness, genetic aspects (parental origin of the extra X chromosome, CAGn repeat length, and pattern of X inactivation), and previous testosterone treatment on cognition. We studied 50 boys with KS (4.1–17.8 years). There was a significant increase in left‐handedness (P = 0.002). Specific language, academic, attentional, and motor abilities tended to be impaired. In the language domain, there was relative sparing of vocabulary and meaningful language understanding abilities but impairment of higher level linguistic competence. KS boys demonstrated an array of motor difficulties, especially in strength and running speed. Deficits in the ability to sustain attention without impulsivity were present in the younger boys. Neither genetic factors examined nor previous testosterone treatment accounted for variation in the cognitive phenotype in KS. The cognitive results from this large KS cohort may be related to atypical brain lateralization and have important diagnostic and psychoeducational implications. The difficulty in complex language processing, impaired attention and motor function in boys with KS may be missed. It is critical that boys with KS are provided with appropriate educational support that targets their learning challenges in school in addition to modifications that address their particular learning style. These findings would also be an important component of counseling clinicians and families about this disorder.


American Journal of Medical Genetics | 1998

Transition to young adulthood in Ullrich-Turner syndrome: Neurodevelopmental changes

Sarah M. Romans; Gerry A. Stefanatos; David P. Roeltgen; Harvey Kushner; Judith L. Ross

Studies describing the neurocognitive profile of Ullrich-Turner syndrome (UTS) have focused primarily on neurodevelopmental changes in childhood and adolescence or in adults with UTS. The objective of the present study was to describe neurodevelopmental changes that occur in UTS females during the transition from adolescence to young-adulthood. The subjects included 99 females with UTS and 89 normal female controls matched for age and socioeconomic status. Subjects were between the ages of 13 and 21 years. All subjects received a battery of neurocognitive tests designed to assess general cognitive ability, academic achievement, memory, language, executive function, visual-spatial/perceptual and motor skills, affect recognition, attention, and motor skills. Results from our study indicated that females with UTS performed significantly less well than controls on measures of spatial/perceptual skills, visual-motor integration, affect recognition, visual memory, attentional abilities, and executive function, consistent with previous reports of cognitive abilities in adolescent UTS females. Moreover, our results indicate that decreased performance in some of these areas persists through late adolescence and into early adulthood while improvement occurs in other areas. It is possible that catch-up in certain cognitive deficiencies in UTS females represents a maturational/developmental lag. Alternatively, the neurodevelopmental changes that were observed in UTS females may result from the cumulative effects of estrogen replacement therapy during adolescence. Therapeutic interventions specific to the demands of young-adulthood are also discussed.


Hormone Research in Paediatrics | 2006

Cognition and the Sex Chromosomes: Studies in Turner Syndrome

Judith L. Ross; David P. Roeltgen; Andrew R. Zinn

Turner syndrome (TS) is a human genetic disorder involving females who lack all or part of one X chromosome. The complex phenotype includes ovarian failure, a characteristic neurocognitive profile and typical physical features. TS features are associated not only with complete monosomy X but also with partial deletions of either the short (Xp) or long (Xq) arm (partial monosomy X). Impaired visual-spatial/perceptual abilities are characteristic of TS children and adults of varying races and socioeconomic status, but global developmental delay is uncommon. The cognitive phenotype generally includes normal verbal function with relatively impaired visual-spatial ability, attention, working memory, and spatially dependent executive function. The constellation of neurocognitive deficits observed in TS is most likely multifactorial and related to a complex interaction between genetic abnormalities and hormonal deficiencies. Furthermore, other determinants, including an additional genetic mechanism, imprinting, may also contribute to cognitive deficits associated with monosomy X. As a relatively common genetic disorder with well-defined manifestations, TS presents an opportunity to investigate genetic and hormonal factors that influence female cognitive development. TS is an excellent model for such studies because of its prevalence, the well-characterized phenotype, and the wealth of molecular resources available for the X chromosome. In the current review, we summarize the hormonal and genetic factors that may contribute to the TS neurocognitive phenotype. The hormonal determinants of cognition in TS are related to estrogen and androgen deficiency. Our genetic hypothesis is that haploinsufficiency for gene/genes on the short arm of the X chromosome (Xp) is responsible for the hallmark features of the TS cognitive phenotype. Careful clinical and molecular characterization of adult subjects missing part of Xp links the TS phenotype of impaired visual spatial/perceptual ability to specific distal Xp chromosome regions. We demonstrate that small, nonmosaic deletion of the distal short arm of the X chromosome in adult women is associated with the same hallmark cognitive profile seen in adult women with TS. Future studies will elucidate the cognitive deficits and the underlying etiology. These results should allow us to begin to design cognitive interventions that might lessen those deficits in the TS population.


American Journal of Human Genetics | 2000

The Turner syndrome-associated neurocognitive phenotype maps to distal Xp

Judith L. Ross; David P. Roeltgen; Harvey Kushner; Fanglin Wei; Andrew R. Zinn

Turner syndrome (TS) is associated with a characteristic neurocognitive profile that includes impaired visuospatial/perceptual abilities. We used a molecular approach to identify a critical region of the X chromosome for neurocognitive aspects of TS. Partial deletions of Xp in 34 females were mapped by FISH or by loss of heterozygosity of polymorphic markers. Discriminant function analysis optimally identified the TS-associated neurocognitive phenotype. Only subjects missing approximately 10 Mb of distal Xp manifested the specified neurocognitive profile. The phenotype was seen with either paternally or maternally inherited deletions and with either complete or incomplete skewing of X inactivation. Fine mapping of informative deletions implicated a critical region of <2 Mb within the pseudoautosomal region (PAR1). We conclude that haploinsufficiency of PAR1 gene(s) is the basis for susceptibility to the TS neurocognitive phenotype.


Pediatrics | 2012

Behavioral and Social Phenotypes in Boys With 47,XYY Syndrome or 47,XXY Klinefelter Syndrome

Judith L. Ross; David P. Roeltgen; Harvey Kushner; Andrew R. Zinn; Allan L. Reiss; Martha Bardsley; Elizabeth McCauley; Nicole Tartaglia

OBJECTIVE: To contrast the behavioral and social phenotypes including a screen for autistic behaviors in boys with 47,XYY syndrome (XYY) or 47,XXY Klinefelter syndrome (KS) and controls and investigate the effect of prenatal diagnosis on the phenotype. METHODS: Patients included 26 boys with 47,XYY, 82 boys with KS, and 50 control boys (ages 4–15 years). Participants and parents completed a physical examination, behavioral questionnaires, and intellectual assessments. RESULTS: Most boys with XYY or KS had Child Behavior Checklist parental ratings within the normal range. On the Child Behavior Checklist, mean problem behaviors t scores were higher in the XYY versus KS groups for the Problem Behavior, Externalizing, Withdrawn, Thought Problems, and Attention Problems subscales. On the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale–Revised, the XYY versus KS group had increased frequency of hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (P < .006). In addition, 50% and 12% of the XYY and KS groups, respectively, had scores >15 for autism screening from the Social Communication Questionnaire. For the boys with KS, prenatal diagnosis was associated with fewer problem behaviors. CONCLUSIONS: A subset of the XYY and KS groups had behavioral difficulties that were more severe in the XYY group. These findings could guide clinical practice and inform patients and parents. Boys diagnosed with XYY or KS should receive a comprehensive psychoeducational evaluation and be screened for learning disabilities, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and autism spectrum disorders.


Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews | 2009

An extra X or Y chromosome: contrasting the cognitive and motor phenotypes in childhood in boys with 47,XYY syndrome or 47,XXY Klinefelter syndrome

Judith L. Ross; Martha P.D. Zeger; Harvey Kushner; Andrew R. Zinn; David P. Roeltgen

OBJECTIVE The goal of this study was to contrast the cognitive phenotypes in boys with 47,XYY (XYY) karyotype and boys with 47,XXY karyotype [Klinefelter syndrome, (KS)], who share an extra copy of the X-Y pseudoautosomal region but differ in their dosage of strictly sex-linked genes. METHODS Neuropsychological evaluation of general cognitive ability, language, memory, attention, visual-spatial abilities, visual-motor skills, and motor function. RESULTS Study cohort: 21 boys with 47,XYY and 93 boys with 47,XXY (KS), age 4-17 years, and 36 age-matched control boys. Both the XYY and KS groups performed less well, on average, than the controls on tests of general cognitive ability, achievement, language, verbal memory, some aspects of attention, and executive function, and motor function. The boys with XYY on average had more severe and pervasive language impairment, at both simple and complex levels, and the boys with KS on average had greater motor impairment in gross motor function and coordination, especially in running speed and agility. CONCLUSIONS The results from these large XYY and KS cohorts have important neurocognitive and educational implications. From the neurocognitive standpoint, the presenting findings afford an opportunity to gain insights into brain development in boys with XYY and those with KS. From the educational standpoint, it is critical that boys with XYY or KS receive appropriate educational interventions that target their specific learning challenges. These findings also provide important information for counseling clinicians and families about these disorders.


Behavioral and Brain Functions | 2007

A turner syndrome neurocognitive phenotype maps to Xp22.3

Andrew R. Zinn; David P. Roeltgen; Gerry A. Stefanatos; Purita Ramos; Frederick F. Elder; Harvey Kushner; Karen Kowal; Judith L. Ross

BackgroundTurner syndrome (TS) is associated with a neurocognitive phenotype that includes selective nonverbal deficits, e.g., impaired visual-spatial abilities. We previously reported evidence that this phenotype results from haploinsufficiency of one or more genes on distal Xp. This inference was based on genotype/phenotype comparisons of individual girls and women with partial Xp deletions, with the neurocognitive phenotype considered a dichotomous trait. We sought to confirm our findings in a large cohort (n = 47) of adult women with partial deletions of Xp or Xq, enriched for subjects with distal Xp deletions.MethodsSubjects were recruited from North American genetics and endocrinology clinics. Phenotype assessment included measures of stature, ovarian function, and detailed neurocognitive testing. The neurocognitive phenotype was measured as a quantitative trait, the Turner Syndrome Cognitive Summary (TSCS) score, derived from discriminant function analysis. Genetic analysis included karyotyping, X inactivation studies, fluorescent in situ hybridization, microsatellite marker genotyping, and array comparative genomic hybridization.ResultsWe report statistical evidence that deletion of Xp22.3, an interval containing 31 annotated genes, is sufficient to cause the neurocognitive phenotype described by the TSCS score. Two other cardinal TS features, ovarian failure and short stature, as well as X chromosome inactivation pattern and subjects age, were unrelated to the TSCS score.ConclusionDetailed mapping suggests that haploinsufficiency of one or more genes in Xp22.3, the distal 8.3 megabases (Mb) of the X chromosome, is responsible for a TS neurocognitive phenotype. This interval includes the 2.6 Mb Xp-Yp pseudoautosomal region (PAR1). Haploinsufficiency of the short stature gene SHOX in PAR1 probably does not cause this TS neurocognitive phenotype. Two genes proximal to PAR1 within the 8.3 Mb critical region, STS and NLGN4X, are attractive candidates for this neurocognitive phenotype.


Pediatric Neurology | 1996

Developmental changes in motor function in girls with Turner syndrome

Judith L. Ross; Harvey Kushner; David P. Roeltgen

The Turner syndrome phenotype is characterized by a particular neurocognitive profile of normal verbal skills, impaired visuospatial and/or visuoperceptual abilities, and difficulty with motor function. We investigated motor function in non-estrogen-treated girls (ages 7-9 and 10-12 years) with Turner syndrome and age-matched female controls. Our goal was to delineate the differences in motor performance between girls with Turner syndrome (n = 78) and control girls (n = 145). Cognitive and motor tasks were administered, as well as nonspatial, repetitive motor tasks, and spatially mediated motor tasks. Questionnaires were also administered. Turner subjects performed less well than the controls on the motor tasks with the greatest spatial demands, particularly in the older age group (age 10-12.9 years). The older control group, unlike the older Turner syndrome group, had significantly increased speed on most of the motor tasks, suggesting a Turner syndrome-associated deficiency in motoric development. The superior performance of the dominant (right) versus the nondominant (left) hand was similar for the Turner syndrome and control groups. In general, the girls with Turner syndrome had evidence of a decreased sense of athletic ability and physical self-image. A likely explanation for motoric deficiency in the older Turner syndrome group relates to their gonadal dysgenesis and estrogen deficiency. In addition to the obvious physical benefits of estrogen replacement, estrogen treatment may have a positive impact on motor function; this will be the subject of future investigations.


Research in Developmental Disabilities | 2012

Social deficits in male children and adolescents with sex chromosome aneuploidy: a comparison of XXY, XYY, and XXYY syndromes.

Lisa Cordeiro; Nicole Tartaglia; David P. Roeltgen; Judith L. Ross

We compare social skills in three groups of males with sex chromosome aneuploidies (SCAs) using the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). Participants included males with XXY (N=102, M=10.08 years), XYY (N=40, M=9.93 years), and XXYY (N=32, M=11.57 years). XXY had lower (better) SRS scores compared to XYY and XXYY. Scores were not significantly different between XYY and XXYY. In all groups, there were significantly more with SRS scores in the severe range compared to the SRS normative sample. All groups scored lowest (better) on Social Motivation. Relationships between SRS scores and demographic and clinical variables were examined. Results describe the social skills in males with SCA, and suggest that an additional Y chromosome may contribute to increased risk of autistic behaviors.


Behavioural Brain Research | 1994

Task persistence and learning ability in normal and chronic low dose MPTP-treated monkeys

David P. Roeltgen; J.S. Schneider

Monkeys exposed to low doses of the dopamine neurotoxin 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) develop cognitive deficits in the absence of gross motor dysfunction. Attentional deficits and task impersistence are now also described in these animals. The task impersistence correlated with no-response errors (i.e. errors of omission) on a delayed response task and improved with dopamine agonist therapy. In parallel studies, it was observed that there were significant differences in the ability of normal monkeys to learn to perform cognitive tasks. We found that monkeys classified as poor learners had similar deficits in task persistence as did MPTP-exposed monkeys, suggesting a relationship between poor cognitive performance and task impersistence in untreated as well as MPTP-treated monkeys. The possible significance of these results for two clinical disorders, early Parkinsons disease and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is discussed. Cognitive and behavioral similarities between chronic low dose MPTP-treated monkeys, early Parkinsons disease patients and people with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder may suggest the existence of related pathophysiological mechanisms in these disorders.

Collaboration


Dive into the David P. Roeltgen's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Judith L. Ross

Thomas Jefferson University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Andrew R. Zinn

University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gerry A. Stefanatos

Albert Einstein Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gordon B. Cutler

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Fumiko Hoeft

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Karen Kowal

Thomas Jefferson University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Nicole Tartaglia

University of Colorado Denver

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Penelope Feuillan

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge