Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Delphine De Smet is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Delphine De Smet.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2016

Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations

Joshua M. Tybur; Yoel Inbar; Lene Aarøe; Pat Barclay; Fiona Kate Barlowe; Mícheál de Barra; D. Vaughn Beckerh; Leah Borovoi; Incheol Choi; Jong An Choik; Nathan S. Consedine; Alan Conway; Jane Rebecca Conway; Paul Conway; Vera Cubela Adoric; Dilara Ekin Demirci; Ana María Fernández; Diogo Conque Seco Ferreirat; Keiko Ishii; Ivana Jakšic; Tingting Ji; Florian van Leeuwen; David M.G. Lewis; Norman P. Li; Jason C. McIntyre; Sumitava Mukherjee; Justin H. Park; Boguslaw Pawlowski; Michael Bang Petersen; David A. Pizarro

Significance Pathogens, and antipathogen behavioral strategies, affect myriad aspects of human behavior. Recent findings suggest that antipathogen strategies relate to political attitudes, with more ideologically conservative individuals reporting more disgust toward pathogen cues, and with higher parasite stress nations being, on average, more conservative. However, no research has yet adjudicated between two theoretical accounts proposed to explain these relationships between pathogens and politics. We find that national parasite stress and individual disgust sensitivity relate more strongly to adherence to traditional norms than they relate to support for barriers between social groups. These results suggest that the relationship between pathogens and politics reflects intragroup motivations more than intergroup motivations. People who are more avoidant of pathogens are more politically conservative, as are nations with greater parasite stress. In the current research, we test two prominent hypotheses that have been proposed as explanations for these relationships. The first, which is an intragroup account, holds that these relationships between pathogens and politics are based on motivations to adhere to local norms, which are sometimes shaped by cultural evolution to have pathogen-neutralizing properties. The second, which is an intergroup account, holds that these same relationships are based on motivations to avoid contact with outgroups, who might pose greater infectious disease threats than ingroup members. Results from a study surveying 11,501 participants across 30 nations are more consistent with the intragroup account than with the intergroup account. National parasite stress relates to traditionalism (an aspect of conservatism especially related to adherence to group norms) but not to social dominance orientation (SDO; an aspect of conservatism especially related to endorsements of intergroup barriers and negativity toward ethnic and racial outgroups). Further, individual differences in pathogen-avoidance motives (i.e., disgust sensitivity) relate more strongly to traditionalism than to SDO within the 30 nations.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2016

Detecting affiliation in colaughter across 24 societies

Gregory A. Bryant; Daniel M. T. Fessler; Riccardo Fusaroli; Edward K. Clint; Lene Aarøe; Coren L. Apicella; Michael Bang Petersen; Shaneikiah T. Bickham; Alexander H. Bolyanatz; Brenda Lía Chávez; Delphine De Smet; Cinthya Díaz; Jana Fančovičová; Michal Fux; Paulina Giraldo-Perez; Anning Hu; Shanmukh V. Kamble; Tatsuya Kameda; Norman P. Li; Francesca R. Luberti; Pavol Prokop; Katinka Quintelier; Brooke A. Scelza; HyunJung Shin; Montserrat Soler; Stefan Stieger; Wataru Toyokawa; Ellis A. van den Hende; Hugo Viciana-Asensio; Saliha Elif Yildizhan

Significance Human cooperation requires reliable communication about social intentions and alliances. Although laughter is a phylogenetically conserved vocalization linked to affiliative behavior in nonhuman primates, its functions in modern humans are not well understood. We show that judges all around the world, hearing only brief instances of colaughter produced by pairs of American English speakers in real conversations, are able to reliably identify friends and strangers. Participants’ judgments of friendship status were linked to acoustic features of laughs known to be associated with spontaneous production and high arousal. These findings strongly suggest that colaughter is universally perceivable as a reliable indicator of relationship quality, and contribute to our understanding of how nonverbal communicative behavior might have facilitated the evolution of cooperation. Laughter is a nonverbal vocal expression that often communicates positive affect and cooperative intent in humans. Temporally coincident laughter occurring within groups is a potentially rich cue of affiliation to overhearers. We examined listeners’ judgments of affiliation based on brief, decontextualized instances of colaughter between either established friends or recently acquainted strangers. In a sample of 966 participants from 24 societies, people reliably distinguished friends from strangers with an accuracy of 53–67%. Acoustic analyses of the individual laughter segments revealed that, across cultures, listeners’ judgments were consistently predicted by voicing dynamics, suggesting perceptual sensitivity to emotionally triggered spontaneous production. Colaughter affords rapid and accurate appraisals of affiliation that transcend cultural and linguistic boundaries, and may constitute a universal means of signaling cooperative relationships.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2017

Cross cultural regularities in the cognitive architecture of pride

Daniel Sznycer; Laith Al-Shawaf; Yoella Bereby-Meyer; Oliver Curry; Delphine De Smet; Elsa Ermer; Sangin Kim; Sunhwa Kim; Norman P. Li; Maria Florencia Lopez Seal; Jennifer McClung; Jiaqing O; Yohsuke Ohtsubo; Tadeg Quillien; Max Schaub; Aaron Nathaniel Sell; Florian van Leeuwen; Leda Cosmides; John Tooby

Significance Cross-cultural tests from 16 nations were performed to evaluate the hypothesis that the emotion of pride evolved to guide behavior to elicit valuation and respect from others. Ancestrally, enhanced evaluations would have led to increased assistance and deference from others. To incline choice, the pride system must compute for a potential action an anticipated pride intensity that tracks the magnitude of the approval or respect that the action would generate in the local audience. All tests demonstrated that pride intensities measured in each location closely track the magnitudes of others’ positive evaluations. Moreover, different cultures echo each other both in what causes pride and in what elicits positive evaluations, suggesting that the underlying valuation systems are universal. Pride occurs in every known culture, appears early in development, is reliably triggered by achievements and formidability, and causes a characteristic display that is recognized everywhere. Here, we evaluate the theory that pride evolved to guide decisions relevant to pursuing actions that enhance valuation and respect for a person in the minds of others. By hypothesis, pride is a neurocomputational program tailored by selection to orchestrate cognition and behavior in the service of: (i) motivating the cost-effective pursuit of courses of action that would increase others’ valuations and respect of the individual, (ii) motivating the advertisement of acts or characteristics whose recognition by others would lead them to enhance their evaluations of the individual, and (iii) mobilizing the individual to take advantage of the resulting enhanced social landscape. To modulate how much to invest in actions that might lead to enhanced evaluations by others, the pride system must forecast the magnitude of the evaluations the action would evoke in the audience and calibrate its activation proportionally. We tested this prediction in 16 countries across 4 continents (n = 2,085), for 25 acts and traits. As predicted, the pride intensity for a given act or trait closely tracks the valuations of audiences, local (mean r = +0.82) and foreign (mean r = +0.75). This relationship is specific to pride and does not generalize to other positive emotions that coactivate with pride but lack its audience-recalibrating function.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2016

Coresidence duration and cues of maternal investment regulate sibling altruism across cultures.

Daniel Sznycer; Delphine De Smet; Joseph Billingsley; Debra Lieberman

Genetic relatedness is a fundamental determinant of social behavior across species. Over the last few decades, researchers have been investigating the proximate psychological mechanisms that enable humans to assess their genetic relatedness to others. Much of this work has focused on identifying cues that predicted relatedness in ancestral environments and examining how they regulate kin-directed behaviors. Despite progress, many basic questions remain unanswered. Here we address three of these questions. First, we examine the replicability of the effect of two association-based cues to relatedness-maternal perinatal association (MPA) and coresidence duration-on sibling-directed altruism. MPA, the observation of a newborn being cared for by ones mother, strongly signals relatedness, but is only available to the older sibling in a sib-pair. Younger siblings, to whom the MPA cue is not available, appear to fall back on the duration of their coresidence with an older sibling. Second, we determine whether the effects of MPA and coresidence duration on sibling-directed altruism obtain across cultures. Last, we explore whether paternal perinatal association (PPA) informs sibship. Data from six studies conducted in California, Hawaii, Dominica, Belgium, and Argentina support past findings regarding the role of MPA and coresidence duration as cues to siblingship. By contrast, PPA had no effect on altruism. We report on levels of altruism toward full, half, and step siblings, and discuss the role alternate cues might play in discriminating among these types of siblings. (PsycINFO Database Record


Thinking & Reasoning | 2012

The case of the drunken sailor: On the generalisable wrongness of harmful transgressions

Katinka Quintelier; Daniel M. T. Fessler; Delphine De Smet

There is a widespread conviction that people distinguish two kinds of acts: on the one hand, acts that are generalisably wrong because they go against universal principles of harm, justice, or rights; on the other hand, acts that are variably right or wrong depending on the social context. In this paper we criticise existing methods that measure generalisability. We report new findings indicating that a modification of generalisability measures is in order. We discuss our findings in light of recent criticisms of moral/conventional research.


Annals of Human Biology | 2012

Why humans do not make good vampires: testing the ability of humans to detect true blood

Delphine De Smet; Linda Van Speybroeck; Jan Verplaetse

Background: Research indicating the effects of real blood or of its iconic representation on human behaviour has thus far concentrated on phobia and aggressiveness. Little is known about other responses or, more fundamentally, about the biological basis of all such responses. Aim: In this study it is examined whether or not humans are able to detect real blood. Methods: Human subjects (n = 89) were asked to distinguish different kinds of blood from red control fluids under varying visual and choice conditions. Relevant differences between subjects were tested for through written questionnaires, including standardized scales for disgust sensitivity (DS-R) and blood phobia (MBPI) and performance on two clinical olfactory tests. Results: Analysis of variance shows that humans are excellent detectors of animal blood (in casu pig blood), whereas the ability of detecting human blood is much less developed. Surprisingly, differences in olfactory capacities and personal experience with blood have no effect on blood detection, while blood fear lowers and disgust sensitivity ameliorates this performance. Conclusion: This study allows further mapping of the exact role of disgust sensitivity in human behaviour, as well as a deliberate choice of materials in blood-related experiments. It is imperative for further research on the behavioural and psychological impact ‘blood’ resorts on humans.


Evolution and Human Behavior | 2014

The Westermarck effect revisited: a psychophysiological study of sibling incest aversion in young female adults☆

Delphine De Smet; Linda Van Speybroeck; Jan Verplaetse


Computers in Human Behavior | 2016

Mental beliefs about blood, and not its smell, affect presence in a violent computer game

Jan Verplaetse; Delphine De Smet


Advances in experimental moral psychology | 2014

Agent versus appraiser moral relativism: an exploratory study

Katinka Quintelier; Delphine De Smet; Daniel M. T. Fessler


NOTAE PRAEHISTORICAE | 2011

The basic prey model in 'deep time': the exploitation of animal resources during MIS 3-5e in France

Dieter Jehs; Delphine De Smet

Collaboration


Dive into the Delphine De Smet's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Norman P. Li

Singapore Management University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel Sznycer

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge