Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Dick de Haan is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Dick de Haan.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2002

Audiogram of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) measured with narrow-band frequency-modulated signals

Ronald A. Kastelein; Paulien Bunskoek; Monique Hagedoorn; Whitlow W. L. Au; Dick de Haan

The underwater hearing sensitivity of a two-year-old harbor porpoise was measured in a pool using standard psycho-acoustic techniques. The go/no-go response paradigm and up-down staircase psychometric method were used. Auditory sensitivity was measured by using narrow-band frequency-modulated signals having center frequencies between 250 Hz and 180 kHz. The resulting audiogram was U-shaped with the range of best hearing (defined as 10 dB within maximum sensitivity) from 16 to 140 kHz, with a reduced sensitivity around 64 kHz. Maximum sensitivity (about 33 dB re 1 microPa) occurred between 100 and 140 kHz. This maximum sensitivity range corresponds with the peak frequency of echolocation pulses produced by harbor porpoises (120-130 kHz). Sensitivity falls about 10 dB per octave below 16 kHz and falls off sharply above 140 kHz (260 dB per octave). Compared to a previous audiogram of this species (Andersen, 1970), the present audiogram shows less sensitive hearing between 2 and 8 kHz and more sensitive hearing between 16 and 180 kHz. This harbor porpoise has the highest upper-frequency limit of all odontocetes investigated. The time it took for the porpoise to move its head 22 cm after the signal onset (movement time) was also measured. It increased from about 1 s at 10 dB above threshold, to about 1.5 s at threshold.


Marine Environmental Research | 2008

Startle response of captive North Sea fish species to underwater tones between 0.1 and 64 kHz.

Ronald A. Kastelein; Sander van der Heul; Willem C. Verboom; Nancy Jennings; Jan van der Veen; Dick de Haan

World-wide, underwater background noise levels are increasing due to anthropogenic activities. Little is known about the effects of anthropogenic noise on marine fish, and information is needed to predict any negative effects. Behavioural startle response thresholds were determined for eight marine fish species, held in a large tank, to tones of 0.1-64 kHz. Response threshold levels varied per frequency within and between species. For sea bass, the 50% reaction threshold occurred for signals of 0.1-0.7 kHz, for thicklip mullet 0.4-0.7 kHz, for pout 0.1-0.25 kHz, for horse mackerel 0.1-2 kHz and for Atlantic herring 4 kHz. For cod, pollack and eel, no 50% reaction thresholds were reached. Reaction threshold levels increased from approximately 100 dB (re 1 microPa, rms) at 0.1 kHz to approximately 160 dB at 0.7 kHz. The 50% reaction thresholds did not run parallel to the hearing curves. This shows that fish species react very differently to sound, and that generalisations about the effects of sound on fish should be made with care. As well as on the spectrum and level of anthropogenic sounds, the reactions of fish probably depend on the context (e.g. location, temperature, physiological state, age, body size, and school size).


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2005

Receiving beam patterns in the horizontal plane of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena)

Ronald A. Kastelein; Mirjam Janssen; Willem C. Verboom; Dick de Haan

Receiving beam patterns of a harbor porpoise were measured in the horizontal plane, using narrow-band frequency modulated signals with center frequencies of 16, 64, and 100 kHz. Total signal duration was 1000 ms, including a 200 ms rise time and 300 ms fall time. The harbor porpoise was trained to participate in a psychophysical test and stationed itself horizontally in a specific direction in the center of a 16-m-diameter circle consisting of 16 equally-spaced underwater transducers. The animals head and the transducers were in the same horizontal plane, 1.5 m below the water surface. The go/no-go response paradigm was used; the animal left the listening station when it heard a sound signal. The method of constants was applied. For each transducer the 50% detection threshold amplitude was determined in 16 trials per amplitude, for each of the three frequencies. The beam patterns were not symmetrical with respect to the midline of the animals body, but had a deflection of 3-7 degrees to the right. The receiving beam pattern narrowed with increasing frequency. Assuming that the pattern is rotation-symmetrical according to an average of the horizontal beam pattern halves, the receiving directivity indices are 4.3 at 16 kHz, 6.0 at 64 kHz, and 11.7 dB at 100 kHz. The receiving directivity indices of the porpoise were lower than those measured for bottlenose dolphins. This means that harbor porpoises have wider receiving beam patterns than bottlenose dolphins for the same frequencies. Directivity of hearing improves the signal-to-noise ratio and thus is a tool for a better detection of certain signals in a given ambient noise condition.


PLOS ONE | 2012

Common Sole Larvae Survive High Levels of Pile-Driving Sound in Controlled Exposure Experiments

L.J. Bolle; Christ A. F. de Jong; S.M. Bierman; Pieter J. G. van Beek; Olvin A. van Keeken; Peter Wessels; Cindy J. G. van Damme; H.V. Winter; Dick de Haan; René P. A. Dekeling

In view of the rapid extension of offshore wind farms, there is an urgent need to improve our knowledge on possible adverse effects of underwater sound generated by pile-driving. Mortality and injuries have been observed in fish exposed to loud impulse sounds, but knowledge on the sound levels at which (sub-)lethal effects occur is limited for juvenile and adult fish, and virtually non-existent for fish eggs and larvae. A device was developed in which fish larvae can be exposed to underwater sound. It consists of a rigid-walled cylindrical chamber driven by an electro-dynamical sound projector. Samples of up to 100 larvae can be exposed simultaneously to a homogeneously distributed sound pressure and particle velocity field. Recorded pile-driving sounds could be reproduced accurately in the frequency range between 50 and 1000 Hz, at zero to peak pressure levels up to 210 dB re 1µPa2 (zero to peak pressures up to 32 kPa) and single pulse sound exposure levels up to 186 dB re 1µPa2s. The device was used to examine lethal effects of sound exposure in common sole (Solea solea) larvae. Different developmental stages were exposed to various levels and durations of pile-driving sound. The highest cumulative sound exposure level applied was 206 dB re 1µPa2s, which corresponds to 100 strikes at a distance of 100 m from a typical North Sea pile-driving site. The results showed no statistically significant differences in mortality between exposure and control groups at sound exposure levels which were well above the US interim criteria for non-auditory tissue damage in fish. Although our findings cannot be extrapolated to fish larvae in general, as interspecific differences in vulnerability to sound exposure may occur, they do indicate that previous assumptions and criteria may need to be revised.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2003

Audiogram of a striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba)

Ronald A. Kastelein; Monique Hagedoorn; Whitlow W. L. Au; Dick de Haan

The underwater hearing sensitivity of a striped dolphin was measured in a pool using standard psycho-acoustic techniques. The go/no-go response paradigm and up-down staircase psychometric method were used. Auditory sensitivity was measured by using 12 narrow-band frequency-modulated signals having center frequencies between 0.5 and 160 kHz. The 50% detection threshold was determined for each frequency. The resulting audiogram for this animal was U-shaped, with hearing capabilities from 0.5 to 160 kHz (8 1/3 oct). Maximum sensitivity (42 dB re 1 microPa) occurred at 64 kHz. The range of most sensitive hearing (defined as the frequency range with sensitivities within 10 dB of maximum sensitivity) was from 29 to 123 kHz (approximately 2 oct). The animals hearing became less sensitive below 32 kHz and above 120 kHz. Sensitivity decreased by about 8 dB per octave below 1 kHz and fell sharply at a rate of about 390 dB per octave above 140 kHz.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2008

Behavioral avoidance threshold level of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) for a continuous 50 kHz pure tone (L)

Ronald A. Kastelein; Willem C. Verboom; Nancy Jennings; Dick de Haan

The use of ultrasonic sounds in alarms for gillnets may be advantageous, but the deterring effects of ultrasound on porpoises are not well understood. Therefore a harbor porpoise in a large floating pen was subjected to a continuous 50 kHz pure tone with a source level of 122+/-3 dB (re 1 microPa, rms). When the test signal was switched on during test periods, the animal moved away from the sound source. Its respiration rate was similar to that during baseline periods, when the sound was switched off. The behavior of the porpoise was related to the sound pressure level distribution in the pen. The sound level at the animals average swimming location during the test periods was approximately 107+/-3 dB (re 1 microPa, rms). The avoidance threshold sound pressure level for a continuous 50 kHz pure tone for this porpoise, in the context of this study, is estimated to be 108+/-3 dB (re 1 microPa, rms). This study demonstrates that porpoises may be deterred from an area by high frequency sounds that are not typically audible to fish and pinnipeds and would be less likely masked by ambient noise.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2007

The influence of signal parameters on the sound source localization ability of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena)

Ronald A. Kastelein; Dick de Haan; Willem C. Verboom

It is unclear how well harbor porpoises can locate sound sources, and thus can locate acoustic alarms on gillnets. Therefore the ability of a porpoise to determine the location of a sound source was determined. The animal was trained to indicate the active one of 16 transducers in a 16-m-diam circle around a central listening station. The duration and received level of the narrowband frequency-modulated signals (center frequencies 16, 64 and 100 kHz) were varied. The animals localization performance increased when the signal duration increased from 600 to 1000 ms. The lower the received sound pressure level (SPL) of the signal, the harder the animal found it to localize the sound source. When pulse duration was long enough (approximately 1 s) and the received SPLs of the sounds were high (34-50 dB above basic hearing thresholds or 3-15 dB above the theoretical masked detection threshold in the ambient noise condition of the present study), the animal could locate sounds of the three frequencies almost equally well. The porpoise was able to locate sound sources up to 124 degrees to its left or right more easily than sounds from behind it.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2005

Underwater hearing sensitivity of a male and a female Steller sea lion (Eumetopias jubatus).

Ronald A. Kastelein; Robbert van Schie; Wim C. Verboom; Dick de Haan

The unmasked underwater hearing sensitivities of an 8-year-old male and a 7-year-old female Steller sea lion were measured in a pool, by using behavioral psychophysics. The animals were trained with positive reinforcement to respond when they detected an acoustic signal and not to respond when they did not. The signals were narrow-band, frequency-modulated stimuli with a duration of 600 ms and center frequencies ranging from 0.5 to 32 kHz for the male and from 4 to 32 kHz for the female. Detection thresholds at each frequency were measured by varying signal amplitude according to the up-down staircase method. The resulting underwater audiogram (50% detection thresholds) for the male Steller sea lion showed the typical mammalian U-shape. His maximum sensitivity (77 dB re: 1 microPa, rms) occurred at 1 kHz. The range of best hearing (10 dB from the maximum sensitivity) was from 1 to 16 kHz (4 octaves). Higher hearing thresholds (indicating poorer sensitivity) were observed below 1 kHz and above 16 kHz. The maximum sensitivity of the female (73 dB re: 1 microPa, rms) occurred at 25 kHz. Higher hearing thresholds (indicating poorer sensitivity) were observed for signals below 16 kHz and above 25 kHz. At frequencies for which both subjects were tested, hearing thresholds of the male were significantly higher than those of the female. The hearing sensitivity differences between the male and female Steller sea lion in this study may be due to individual differences in sensitivity between the subjects or due to sexual dimorphism in hearing.


Bioacoustics-the International Journal of Animal Sound and Its Recording | 2008

ACOUSTIC DOSE-RESPONSE EFFECTS IN MARINE FISH

Ronald A. Kastelein; Sander van der Heul; Willem C. Verboom; Dick de Haan; Peter Reijnders

Enger, P. S. (1967). Hearing in herring. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 22, 527-538. Holst, J. C., Rottingen, 1., & Melle, W. (2002). The herring. In The Norwegian Sea Ecosystem (Ed. by H. R. Skjoldal), pp. 203-226. Trondheim: Tapir Academic Press. Moore, S. E., Francine, J. K., Bowles, A. E., & Ford, J. K. B. (1988). Analysis of calls of killer whales, Orcinus orca, from Iceland to Norway. Rit Fiskideildar 11, 2252250. Richardson, W. J., Greene, C. R., Jr., Malme, C. 1., & Thomson, D. H. (1995) Marine Mammals and Noise. San Diego: Academic Press. Schevill, W. E., & Watkins, W. A. (1966). Sound structure and directionality in Orcinus (killer whale). Zoologica (N. Y.) 51, 71-76.


Journal of the Acoustical Society of America | 2011

Testing mortality of fish larvae due to simulated offshore piling noise.

Christ A. F. de Jong; Pieter J. G. van Beek; Michael A. Ainslie; L.J. Bolle; Olvin A. van Keeken; Cindy J. G. van Damme; Hendrik V. Winter; Dick de Haan

Driven by the concern that impulsive noise produced by offshore pile driving may lead to mortality of fish larvae, a device was developed for testing the sensitivity of small fish and fish larvae to sound exposure. The device consists of a rigid‐walled cylindrical chamber (110‐mm diameter, 160‐mm height), driven by an electrodynamical sound projector. Samples of up to 100 larvae can be exposed simultaneously to a homogeneously distributed sound pressure and particle velocity field, at a controllable static pressure up to 3 bars. Two configurations are available with either a dominant sound pressure or a dominant particle velocity exposure. Recorded piling noise can be reproduced in a controlled way, in the frequency range between 50 Hz and 1 kHz, at zero to peak pressure up to 40 kPa and single pulse sound exposure levels up to 187 dB re 1 μPa2 s, or peak particle velocity up to 2.2 cm/s and integrated square particle velocity level 124 dB re 1 (nm/s)2 s. Tests are carried out in which sole (Solea solea) ...

Collaboration


Dive into the Dick de Haan's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Cindy J. G. van Damme

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

L.J. Bolle

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

H.V. Winter

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

S.M. Bierman

Wageningen University and Research Centre

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge