Dominique Kessel
Autonomous University of Madrid
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Dominique Kessel.
PLOS ONE | 2012
Luis Carretié; Marcos Ríos; José Antonio Periañez; Dominique Kessel; Juan Álvarez-Linera
Exogenous attention can be understood as an adaptive tool that permits the detection and processing of biologically salient events even when the individual is engaged in a resource-consuming task. Indirect data suggest that the spatial frequency of stimulation may be a crucial element in this process. Behavioral and neural data (both functional and structural) were analyzed for 36 participants engaged in a digit categorization task in which distracters were presented. Distracters were biologically salient or anodyne images, and had three spatial frequency formats: intact, low spatial frequencies only, and high spatial frequencies only. Behavior confirmed enhanced exogenous attention to biologically salient distracters. The activity in the right and left intraparietal sulci and the right middle frontal gyrus was associated with this behavioral pattern and was greater in response to salient than to neutral distracters, the three areas presenting strong correlations to each other. Importantly, the enhanced response of this network to biologically salient distracters with respect to neutral distracters relied on low spatial frequencies to a significantly greater extent than on high spatial frequencies. Structural analyses suggested the involvement of internal capsule, superior longitudinal fasciculus and corpus callosum in this network. Results confirm that exogenous attention is preferentially captured by biologically salient information, and suggest that the architecture and function underlying this process are low spatial frequency-biased.
Neuropsychologia | 2013
Luis Carretié; Jacobo Albert; Sara López-Martín; Sandra Hoyos; Dominique Kessel; Manuel Tapia; Almudena Capilla
Mechanisms underlying exogenous attention to central and peripheral distracters were temporally and spatially explored while 30 participants performed a digit categorization task. Neural (event-related potentials-ERPs-, analyzed both at the scalp and at the voxel level) and behavioral indices of exogenous attention were analyzed. Distracters were either biologically salient or neutral, in order to test whether the exogenous attention bias to the former observed in previous studies is independent of, or interacts with, distracter eccentricity. Two subcomponents of the N2 component of the ERPs, N2olp and N2ft, reflected processes related to peripheral distracters processing. N2olp effects, located in the dorsal attention network (supplementary motor area), were probably related to covert reorientation to peripheral distracters. N2ft effects, located in the default mode network (posterior cingulate cortex), appeared to reflect less effort in the ongoing task when peripheral distracters were presented. N2ft also showed a biological saliency effect which was independent of eccentricity and was located in the polar/ventral prefrontal cortex. P3 showed greater amplitudes to centrally presented distracters. These latter effects were located in TEO (visual cortex), and would be functionally associated with spatial interference between the target and central distracters. Behavior showed the relevance of both central and peripheral distracters in exogenous attention. These results indicate that exogenous attention to peripheral distracters differed in temporal and spatial terms from exogenous attention to central distracters and that it is biased towards biologically salient events irrespective of their eccentricity.
World Journal of Biological Psychiatry | 2015
Preethi Premkumar; Juliana Onwumere; Jacobo Albert; Dominique Kessel; Veena Kumari; Elizabeth Kuipers; Luis Carretié
Abstract Objectives: Schizotypy relates to rejection sensitivity (anxiety reflecting an expectancy of social exclusion) and neuroticism (excessive evaluation of negative emotions). Positive schizotypy (e.g., perceptual aberrations and odd beliefs) and negative schizotypy (e.g., social and physical anhedonia) could relate to altered attention to rejection because of neuroticism. Methods: Forty-one healthy individuals were assessed on positive and negative schizotypy and neuroticism, and event-related potentials during rejecting, accepting and neutral scenes. Participants were categorised into high, moderate and low neuroticism groups. Using temporo-spatial principal components analyses, P200 (peak latency =290 ms) and P300 amplitudes (peak latency = 390 ms) were measured, reflecting mobilisation of attention and early attention, respectively. Results: Scalp-level and cortical source analysis revealed elevated fronto-parietal N300/P300 amplitude and P200-related dorsal anterior cingulate current density during rejection than acceptance/neutral scenes. Positive schizotypy related inversely to parietal P200 amplitude during rejection. Negative schizotypy related positively to P200 middle occipital current density. Negative schizotypy related positively to parietal P300, where the association was stronger in high and moderate, than low, neuroticism groups. Conclusions: Positive and negative schizotypy relate divergently to attention to rejection. Positive schizotypy attenuates, but negative schizotypy increases rejection-related mobilisation of attention. Negative schizotypy increases early attention to rejection partly due to elevated neuroticism.
Biological Psychology | 2016
Almudena Capilla; María Melcón; Dominique Kessel; Rosbén Calderón; Paula Pazo-Álvarez; Luis Carretié
Visual stimulation is frequently employed in electroencephalographic (EEG) research. However, despite its widespread use, no studies have thoroughly evaluated how the morphology of the visual event-related potentials (ERPs) varies according to the spatial location of stimuli. Hence, the purpose of this study was to perform a detailed retinotopic mapping of visual ERPs. We recorded EEG activity while participants were visually stimulated with 60 pattern-reversing checkerboards placed at different polar angles and eccentricities. Our results show five pattern-reversal ERP components. C1 and C2 components inverted polarity between the upper and lower hemifields. P1 and N1 showed higher amplitudes and shorter latencies to stimuli located in the contralateral lower quadrant. In contrast, P2 amplitude was enhanced and its latency was reduced by stimuli presented in the periphery of the upper hemifield. The retinotopic maps presented here could serve as a guide for selecting optimal visuo-spatial locations in future ERP studies.
international symposium on biomedical imaging | 2013
Kenia Martínez; Julio E. Villalon-Reina; Dominique Kessel; Anand A. Joshi; Jose Angel Pineda; Neda Jahanshad; Talia M. Nir; Kristian Eschenburg; Francisco J. Román; Miguel Burgaleta; Ana Beatriz Solana; M. Ángeles Quiroga; Roberto Colom; Paul M. Thompson
Properties of the brains structural networks can be analyzed by applying fiber-tracking techniques and network analysis to diffusion MRI. Here we applied exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to anatomical connectivity matrices, to identify brain networks whose properties predicted higher-order cognitive function. Using diffusion MRI scans from 104 healthy young adults, we computed connectivity matrices based on deterministic and probabilistic tractography (with the FACT and Hough transform methods). Both sets of matrices were submitted to factor analysis, to identify sub-networks relevant for predicting cognitive function. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure and Bartletts sphericity test were used to recover latent factors from the connectivity matrices, and only the Hough method yielded factorable outputs. Factor scores were related to fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence, and processing speed. Middle temporal and lateral prefrontal connectivity measures predicted all cognitive scores, except spatial intelligence. Cognitive performance was not predictable from global connectivity measures, which depended on the tractography method.
Scientific Reports | 2017
Alejandra Carboni; Dominique Kessel; Almudena Capilla; Luis Carretié
The interplay between exogenous attention to emotional distractors and the baseline affective state has not been well established yet. The present study aimed to explore this issue through behavioral measures and event-related potentials (ERPs). Participants (N = 30) completed a digit categorization task depicted over negative, positive or neutral distractor background pictures, while they experienced negative, positive and neutral affective states elicited by movie scenes. Behavioral results showed higher error rates and longer reaction times for negative distractors than for neutral and positive ones, irrespective of the current emotional state. Neural indices showed that the participants’ affective state modulated N1 amplitudes, irrespective of distractor type, while the emotional charge of distractors modulated N2, irrespective of the emotional state. Importantly, an interaction of state and distractor type was observed in LPP. These results demonstrate that exogenous attention to emotional distractors is independent from modulating effects of the emotional baseline state at early, automatic stages of processing. However, attention to emotional distractors and affective state interact at later latencies.
Neuropsychologia | 2017
Sandra C. Soares; Dominique Kessel; María Hernández-Lorca; María J. García-Rubio; Paulo Rodrigues; Nuno Gomes; Luis Carretié
ABSTRACT Research has consistently shown that threat stimuli automatically attract attention in order to activate the defensive response systems. Recent findings have provided evidence that snakes tuned the visual system of evolving primates for their astute detection, particularly under challenging perceptual conditions. The goal of the present study was to measure behavioral and electrophysiological indices of exogenous attention to snakes, compared with spiders – matched for rated fear levels but for which sources of natural selection are less well grounded, and to innocuous animals (birds), which were presented as distracters, while participants were engaged in a letter discrimination task. Duration of stimuli, consisting in a letter string and a concurrent distracter, was either presented for 180 or 360 ms to explore if the stimulus duration was a modulating effect of snakes in capturing attention. Results showed a specific early (P1) exogenous attention‐related brain potential with maximal amplitude to snakes in both durations, which was followed by an enhanced late attention‐related potential (LPP) showing enhanced amplitudes to spiders, particularly under the longer exposure durations. These results suggest that exogenous attention to different classes of threat stimuli follows a gradual process, with the most evolutionary‐driven stimulus, i.e., snakes, being more efficient at attracting early exogenous attention, thus more dependent on bottom‐up processes. HIGHLIGHTSWe measured behavioral and neural indices of attention to snakes and spiders.We used an exogneous attention task and manipulated stimulus duration.Results showed maximal amplitudes in P1 for snakes in both exposure durations.Enhanced LPP amplitudes in longer durations were observed for spiders.Reaction times and error rates showed significant correlations with LPP amplitudes.
Biological Psychology | 2016
Dominique Kessel; María J. García-Rubio; E. Kirstin González; Manuel Tapia; Sara López-Martín; Francisco J. Román; Almudena Capilla; Kenia Martínez; Roberto Colom; Luis Carretié
Memorizing emotional stimuli in a preferential way seems to be one of the adaptive strategies brought on by evolution for supporting survival. However, there is a lack of electrophysiological evidence on this bias in working memory. The present study analyzed the influence of emotion on the updating component of working memory. Behavioral and electrophysiological indices were measured from a 3-back task using negative, neutral, and positive faces. Electrophysiological data evidenced an emotional influence on the working memory sensitive P3 component, which presented larger amplitudes for negative matching faces compared to neutral ones. This effect originated in the superior parietal cortex, previously reported to be involved in N-back tasks. Additionally, P3 results showed a correlation with reaction times, where higher amplitudes were associated with faster responses for negative matching faces. These findings indicate that electrophysiological measures seem to be very suitable indices of the emotional influence on working memory.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2017
Luis Carretié; Dominique Kessel; María J. García-Rubio; Tamara Giménez-Fernández; Sandra Hoyos; María Hernández-Lorca
Exogenous attention is a set of mechanisms that allow us to detect and reorient toward salient events—such as appetitive or aversive—that appear out of the current focus of attention. The nature of these mechanisms, particularly the involvement of the parvocellular and magnocellular visual processing systems, was explored. Thirty-four participants performed a demanding digit categorization task while salient (spiders or S) and neutral (wheels or W) stimuli were presented as distractors under two figure–ground formats: heterochromatic/isoluminant (exclusively processed by the parvocellular system, Par trials) and isochromatic/heteroluminant (preferentially processed by the magnocellular system, Mag trials). This resulted in four conditions: SPar, SMag, WPar, and WMag. Behavioral (RTs and error rates in the task) and electrophysiological (ERPs) indices of exogenous attention were analyzed. Behavior showed greater attentional capture by SMag than by SPar distractors and enhanced modulation of SMag capture as fear of spiders reported by participants increased. ERPs reflected a sequence from magnocellular dominant (P1p, ≃120 msec) to both magnocellular and parvocellular processing (N2p and P2a, ≃200 msec). Importantly, amplitudes in one N2p subcomponent were greater to SMag than to SPar and WMag distractors, indicating greater magnocellular sensitivity to saliency. Taking together, results support a magnocellular bias in exogenous attention toward distractors of any nature during initial processing, a bias that remains in later stages when biologically salient distractors are present.
Personality and Individual Differences | 2014
Kenia Martínez; Julio E. Villalon-Reina; J.A. Pineda; Dominique Kessel; Anand A. Joshi; Kristian Eschenburg; Neda Jahanshad; Francisco J. Román; Miguel Burgaleta; Paul M. Thompson; Roberto Colom
Here white matter fibers connecting several brain regions were obtained with deterministic and probabilistic tractography in a sample of 104 healthy young adults. The properties of the computed connectivity matrices were studied from two distinguishable perspectives. The first was based on global and nodal standard graph theory metrics, whereas the second relied on exploratory factor analysis (EFA) for selecting potentially relevant sub-networks. A broad set of cognitive constructs was considered: general (g), fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence, working memory capacity, attention, and processing speed. The findings show that graph metrics change depending on the applied tractography, and only the probabilistic method yielded factorable connectivity matrices. Secondly, EFA reveals structural networks underlying variability in intelligence factors and cognitive processes. Thirdly, variability in g and Gf was predicted by functional integration–segregation and predominant long-distance structural connections in the brain, playing a key role the left inferior parietal and prefrontal areas.