Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Elizabeth A Gilbert is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Elizabeth A Gilbert.


Perspectives on Psychological Science | 2014

Registered Replication Report

V. K. Alogna; M. K. Attaya; Philip Aucoin; Štěpán Bahník; S. Birch; Angela R Birt; Brian H. Bornstein; Samantha Bouwmeester; Maria A. Brandimonte; Charity Brown; K. Buswell; Curt A. Carlson; Maria A. Carlson; S. Chu; A. Cislak; M. Colarusso; Melissa F. Colloff; Kimberly S. Dellapaolera; Jean-François Delvenne; A. Di Domenico; Aaron Drummond; Gerald Echterhoff; John E. Edlund; Casey Eggleston; B. Fairfield; G. Franco; Fiona Gabbert; B. W. Gamblin; Maryanne Garry; R. Gentry

Trying to remember something now typically improves your ability to remember it later. However, after watching a video of a simulated bank robbery, participants who verbally described the robber were 25% worse at identifying the robber in a lineup than were participants who instead listed U.S. states and capitals—this has been termed the “verbal overshadowing” effect (Schooler & Engstler-Schooler, 1990). More recent studies suggested that this effect might be substantially smaller than first reported. Given uncertainty about the effect size, the influence of this finding in the memory literature, and its practical importance for police procedures, we conducted two collections of preregistered direct replications (RRR1 and RRR2) that differed only in the order of the description task and a filler task. In RRR1, when the description task immediately followed the robbery, participants who provided a description were 4% less likely to select the robber than were those in the control condition. In RRR2, when the description was delayed by 20 min, they were 16% less likely to select the robber. These findings reveal a robust verbal overshadowing effect that is strongly influenced by the relative timing of the tasks. The discussion considers further implications of these replications for our understanding of verbal overshadowing.


Current opinion in psychology | 2016

Current and future directions in culture and happiness research

Shigehiro Oishi; Elizabeth A Gilbert

Once believed to be universal, a growing body of research shows that both the conception and predictors of happiness vary cross-culturally. First, the meaning and importance of happiness varies both across time and between nations. Americans, for instance, tend to define happiness in terms of pleasure or enjoyment and view happiness as universally positive, whereas East Asian and Middle Eastern cultures may highlight the transient and socially disruptive nature of happiness and be ambivalent about whether it is good. Second, predictors of happiness vary between cultures. Recent work highlights new mediators (e.g., relational mobility), individual predictors (e.g., person-culture fit), societal factors (e.g., good governance and wealth), within-culture variations (e.g., at the state or city level), and interventions (e.g., practicing gratitude) that differ cross-culturally or help explain cultural differences in happiness. Though many questions remain, this review highlights how these recent advances broaden and revise our understanding of culture and happiness.


Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 2015

Counterfactuals, Control, and Causation Why Knowledgeable People Get Blamed More

Elizabeth A Gilbert; Elizabeth R. Tenney; Christopher R. Holland; Barbara A. Spellman

Legal and prescriptive theories of blame generally propose that judgments about an actor’s mental state (e.g., her knowledge or intent) should remain separate from judgments about whether the actor caused an outcome. Three experiments, however, show that, even in the absence of intent or immorality, actors who have knowledge relevant to a potential outcome will be rated more causal of that outcome than their ignorant counterparts, even when their actions were identical. Additional analysis revealed that this effect was mediated by counterfactual thinking—that is, by imagining ways the outcome could have been prevented. Specifically, when actors had knowledge, participants generated more counterfactuals about ways the outcome could have been different that the actor could control, which in turn increased causal assignment to the actor. These results are consistent with the Crediting Causality Model, but conflict with some legal and moral theories of blame.


Psychological Inquiry | 2014

Blame, Cause and Counterfactuals: The Inextricable Link

Barbara A. Spellman; Elizabeth A Gilbert


Personality and Individual Differences | 2014

Self–informant agreement for subjective well-being among Japanese

Masao Saeki; Shigehiro Oishi; Takashi Maeno; Elizabeth A Gilbert


Archive | 2013

Immorality or Abnormality: What's to Blame for Increasing Blame?

Elizabeth A Gilbert; Barbara A. Spellman


Archive | 2015

Supplementary Information and Analyses

Elizabeth A Gilbert; Elizabeth R. Tenney


Archive | 2015

Data and codebook for "Counterfactuals, control, and causation: Why knowledgeable people get blamed more"

Elizabeth A Gilbert; Elizabeth R. Tenney


Archive | 2015

Race and SES influence interact when influencing blame judgments

Alexander Guinn; Elizabeth A Gilbert


Archive | 2015

Race and SES Interact to Influence Blame Judgments

Elizabeth A Gilbert; Alexander Guinn; Dick Reppucci

Collaboration


Dive into the Elizabeth A Gilbert's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brian H. Bornstein

University of Nebraska–Lincoln

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kimberly S. Dellapaolera

University of Nebraska–Lincoln

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge