Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin.


NeuroImage | 2013

Controlling automatic imitative tendencies: Interactions between mirror neuron and cognitive control systems

Katy A. Cross; Salvatore Torrisi; Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Marco Iacoboni

Humans have an automatic tendency to imitate others. Although several regions commonly observed in social tasks have been shown to be involved in imitation control, there is little work exploring how these regions interact with one another. We used fMRI and dynamic causal modeling to identify imitation-specific control mechanisms and examine functional interactions between regions. Participants performed a pre-specified action (lifting their index or middle finger) in response to videos depicting the same two actions (biological cues) or dots moving with similar trajectories (non-biological cues). On congruent trials, the stimulus and response were similar (e.g. index finger response to index finger or left side dot stimulus), while on incongruent trials the stimulus and response were dissimilar (e.g. index finger response to middle finger or right side dot stimulus). Reaction times were slower on incongruent compared to congruent trials for both biological and non-biological stimuli, replicating previous findings that suggest the automatic imitative or spatially compatible (congruent) response must be controlled on incongruent trials. Neural correlates of the congruency effects were different depending on the cue type. The medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (IFGpo) and the left anterior insula were involved specifically in controlling imitation. In addition, the IFGpo was also more active for biological compared to non-biological stimuli, suggesting that the region represents the frontal node of the human mirror neuron system (MNS). Effective connectivity analysis exploring the interactions between these regions, suggests a role for the mPFC and ACC in imitative conflict detection and the anterior insula in conflict resolution processes, which may occur through interactions with the frontal node of the MNS. We suggest an extension of the previous models of imitation control involving interactions between imitation-specific and general cognitive control mechanisms.


PLOS ONE | 2008

Left Hemisphere Specialization for Oro-Facial Movements of Learned Vocal Signals by Captive Chimpanzees

Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Jamie L. Russell; Hani D. Freeman; Adrien Meguerditchian; William D. Hopkins

Background The left hemisphere of the human brain is dominant in the production of speech and signed language. Whether similar lateralization of function for communicative signal production is present in other primates remains a topic of considerable debate. In the current study, we examined whether oro-facial movements associated with the production of learned attention-getting sounds are differentially lateralized compared to facial expressions associated with the production of species-typical emotional vocalizations in chimpanzees. Methodology/ Principal Findings Still images captured from digital video were used to quantify oro-facial asymmetries in the production of two attention-getting sounds and two species-typical vocalizations in a sample of captive chimpanzees. Comparisons of mouth asymmetries during production of these sounds revealed significant rightward biased asymmetries for the attention-getting sounds and significant leftward biased asymmetries for the species-typical sounds. Conclusions/Significance These results suggest that the motor control of oro-facial movements associated with the production of learned sounds is lateralized to the left hemisphere in chimpanzees. Furthermore, the findings suggest that the antecedents for lateralization of human speech may have been present in the common ancestor of chimpanzees and humans ∼5 mya and are not unique to the human lineage.


Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience | 2010

Culture and neuroscience: additive or synergistic?

Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Mirella Dapretto; Marco Iacoboni

The investigation of cultural phenomena using neuroscientific methods-cultural neuroscience (CN)-is receiving increasing attention. Yet it is unclear whether the integration of cultural study and neuroscience is merely additive, providing additional evidence of neural plasticity in the human brain, or truly synergistic, yielding discoveries that neither discipline could have achieved alone. We discuss how the parent fields to CN: cross-cultural psychology, psychological anthropology and cognitive neuroscience inform the investigation of the role of cultural experience in shaping the brain. Drawing on well-established methodologies from cross-cultural psychology and cognitive neuroscience, we outline a set of guidelines for CN, evaluate 17 CN studies in terms of these guidelines, and provide a summary table of our results. We conclude that the combination of culture and neuroscience is both additive and synergistic; while some CN methodologies and findings will represent the direct union of information from parent fields, CN studies employing the methodological rigor required by this logistically challenging new field have the potential to transform existing methodologies and produce unique findings.


Ajidd-american Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities | 2009

Abnormal fMRI Activation Pattern during Story Listening in Individuals with Down Syndrome.

Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Susan M. Rivera; Elizabeth D. O'Hare; Elizabeth R. Sowell; Joseph D. Pinter

Down syndrome is characterized by disproportionately severe impairments of speech and language, yet little is known about the neural underpinnings of these deficits. We compared fMRI activation patterns during passive story listening in 9 young adults with Down syndrome and 9 approximately age-matched, typically developing controls. The typically developing group exhibited greater activation than did the Down syndrome group in classical receptive language areas (superior and middle temporal gyri) for forward > backward speech; the Down syndrome group exhibited greater activation in cingulate gyrus, superior and inferior parietal lobules, and precuneus for both forward speech > rest and backward speech > rest. The Down syndrome group showed almost no difference in activation patterns between the language (forward speech) and nonlanguage (backward speech) conditions.


Human Brain Mapping | 2014

Neural processing of race during imitation: Self‐Similarity Versus Social Status

Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Katy A. Cross; Marco Iacoboni; Mirella Dapretto

People preferentially imitate others who are similar to them or have high social status. Such imitative biases are thought to have evolved because they increase the efficiency of cultural acquisition. Here we focused on distinguishing between self‐similarity and social status as two candidate mechanisms underlying neural responses to a persons race during imitation. We used fMRI to measure neural responses when 20 African American (AA) and 20 European American (EA) young adults imitated AA, EA and Chinese American (CA) models and also passively observed their gestures and faces. We found that both AA and EA participants exhibited more activity in lateral frontoparietal and visual regions when imitating AAs compared with EAs or CAs. These results suggest that racial self‐similarity is not likely to modulate neural responses to race during imitation, in contrast with findings from previous neuroimaging studies of face perception and action observation. Furthermore, AA and EA participants associated AAs with lower social status than EAs or CAs, suggesting that the social status associated with different racial groups may instead modulate neural activity during imitation of individuals from those groups. Taken together, these findings suggest that neural responses to race during imitation are driven by socially learned associations rather than self‐similarity. This may reflect the adaptive role of imitation in social learning, where learning from higher status models can be more beneficial. This study provides neural evidence consistent with evolutionary theories of cultural acquisition. Hum Brain Mapp 35:1723–1739, 2014.


Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience | 2012

Own-gender imitation activates the brain's reward circuitry

Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Macro Iacoboni; Alia Martin; Mirella Dapretto

Imitation is an important component of human social learning throughout life. Theoretical models and empirical data from anthropology and psychology suggest that people tend to imitate self-similar individuals, and that such imitation biases increase the adaptive value (e.g., self-relevance) of learned information. It is unclear, however, what neural mechanisms underlie peoples tendency to imitate those similar to themselves. We focused on the own-gender imitation bias, a pervasive bias thought to be important for gender identity development. While undergoing fMRI, participants imitated own- and other-gender actors performing novel, meaningless hand signs; as control conditions, they also simply observed such actions and viewed still portraits of the same actors. Only the ventral and dorsal striatum, orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala were more active when imitating own- compared to other-gender individuals. A Bayesian analysis of the BrainMap neuroimaging database demonstrated that the striatal region preferentially activated by own-gender imitation is selectively activated by classical reward tasks in the literature. Taken together, these findings reveal a neurobiological mechanism associated with the own-gender imitation bias and demonstrate a novel role of reward-processing neural structures in social behavior.


Nature Neuroscience | 2018

Generalizable representations of pain, cognitive control, and negative emotion in medial frontal cortex

Philip A. Kragel; Michiko Kano; Lukas Van Oudenhove; Huynh Giao Ly; Patrick Dupont; Amandine Rubio; Chantal Delon-Martin; Bruno Bonaz; Stephen B. Manuck; Peter J. Gianaros; Marta Ceko; Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Choong Wan Woo; Thomas E. Nichols; Tor D. Wager

The medial frontal cortex, including anterior midcingulate cortex, has been linked to multiple psychological domains, including cognitive control, pain, and emotion. However, it is unclear whether this region encodes representations of these domains that are generalizable across studies and subdomains. Additionally, if there are generalizable representations, do they reflect a single underlying process shared across domains or multiple domain-specific processes? We decomposed multivariate patterns of functional MRI activity from 270 participants across 18 studies into study-specific, subdomain-specific, and domain-specific components and identified latent multivariate representations that generalized across subdomains but were specific to each domain. Pain representations were localized to anterior midcingulate cortex, negative emotion representations to ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and cognitive control representations to portions of the dorsal midcingulate. These findings provide evidence for medial frontal cortex representations that generalize across studies and subdomains but are specific to distinct psychological domains rather than reducible to a single underlying process.Assessing person-level human brain maps across 18 fMRI studies, the authors identify separable representations of pain, cognitive control, and negative emotion in the medial frontal cortex that generalize across different studies and tasks.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2017

The Neural Systems of Forgiveness: An Evolutionary Psychological Perspective

Joseph Billingsley; Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin

Evolution-minded researchers posit that the suite of human cognitive adaptations may include forgiveness systems. According to these researchers, forgiveness systems regulate interpersonal motivation toward a transgressor in the wake of harm by weighing multiple factors that influence both the potential gains of future interaction with the transgressor and the likelihood of future harm. Although behavioral research generally supports this evolutionary model of forgiveness, the model’s claims have not been examined with available neuroscience specifically in mind, nor has recent neuroscientific research on forgiveness generally considered the evolutionary literature. The current review aims to help bridge this gap by using evolutionary psychology and cognitive neuroscience to mutually inform and interrogate one another. We briefly summarize the evolutionary research on forgiveness, then review recent neuroscientific findings on forgiveness in light of the evolutionary model. We emphasize neuroscientific research that links desire for vengeance to reward-based areas of the brain, that singles out prefrontal areas likely associated with inhibition of vengeful feelings, and that correlates the activity of a theory-of-mind network with assessments of the intentions and blameworthiness of those who commit harm. In addition, we identify gaps in the existing neuroscientific literature, and propose future research directions that might address them, at least in part.


bioRxiv | 2018

Multiple brain networks mediating stimulus-pain relationships in humans

Stephan Geuter; Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Mathieu Roy; Lauren Y. Atlas; Liane Schmidt; Anjali Krishnan; Leonie Koban; Tor D. Wager; Martin A. Lindquist

The brain transforms nociceptive input into a complex pain experience comprised of sensory, affective, motivational, and cognitive components. However, it is still unclear how pain arises from nociceptive input, and which brain networks coordinate to generate pain experiences. We introduce a new high-dimensional mediation analysis technique to estimate distributed, network-level patterns mediating the relationship between stimulus intensity and pain. In a large-scale analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data (N=284), we identify both traditional mediators in somatosensory brain regions and additional mediators located in prefrontal, midbrain, striatal, and default-mode regions unrelated to nociception in standard analyses. The whole brain mediators are specific for pain vs. aversive sounds and are organized in five functional networks. Brain mediators explain 32% more within-subject variance of single-trial pain ratings than previous brain-based models. Our results provide a new, broader view of the networks underlying pain experience, as well as distinct targets for interventions.


NeuroImage | 2012

Race modulates neural activity during imitation

Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin; Marco Iacoboni; Alia Martin; Katy A. Cross; Mirella Dapretto

Collaboration


Dive into the Elizabeth A. Reynolds Losin's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marco Iacoboni

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Tor D. Wager

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anjali Krishnan

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Katy A. Cross

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alia Martin

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Choong Wan Woo

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Leonie Koban

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge