Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Emily M. Elliott is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Emily M. Elliott.


Cognitive Psychology | 2005

On the Capacity of Attention: Its Estimation and Its Role in Working Memory and Cognitive Aptitudes

Nelson Cowan; Emily M. Elliott; J. Scott Saults; Candice Coker Morey; Sam Mattox; Anna Hismjatullina; Andrew R. A. Conway

Working memory (WM) is the set of mental processes holding limited information in a temporarily accessible state in service of cognition. We provide a theoretical framework to understand the relation between WM and aptitude measures. The WM measures that have yielded high correlations with aptitudes include separate storage-and-processing task components, on the assumption that WM involves both storage and processing. We argue that the critical aspect of successful WM measures is that rehearsal and grouping processes are prevented, allowing a clearer estimate of how many separate chunks of information the focus of attention circumscribes at once. Storage-and-processing tasks correlate with aptitudes, according to this view, largely because the processing task prevents rehearsal and grouping of items to be recalled. In a developmental study, we document that several scope-of-attention measures that do not include a separate processing component, but nevertheless prevent efficient rehearsal or grouping, also correlate well with aptitudes and with storage-and-processing measures. So does digit span in children too young to rehearse.


Memory & Cognition | 2006

Scope of Attention, Control of Attention, and Intelligence in Children and Adults

Nelson Cowan; Nathanael M. Fristoe; Emily M. Elliott; Ryan P. Brunner; J. Scott Saults

Recent experimentation has shown that cognitive aptitude measures are predicted by tests of the scope of an individual’s attention or capacity in simple working memory tasks and also by the ability to control attention. However, these experiments do not indicate how separate or related the scope and control of attention are. An experiment with 52 children (10 to 11 years old) and 52 college students included measures of the scope and control of attention, as well as verbal and nonverbal aptitude measures. The children showed little evidence of using sophisticated attentional control, but the scope of attention predicted intelligence in that group. In adults, both the scope and control of attention varied among individuals and accounted for considerable individual variance in intelligence. About one third that variance was shared between scope and control, and the rest was unique to one or the other. Scope and control of attention appear to be related but distinct contributors to intelligence.


Child Development | 1999

The Role of Attention in the Development of Short-Term Memory: Age Differences in the Verbal Span of Apprehension

Nelson Cowan; Lara D. Nugent; Emily M. Elliott; Igor Ponomarev; J. Scott Saults

In previous studies of memory span, participants have attended to the stimuli while they were presented, and therefore have had the opportunity to use a variety of mnemonic strategies. In the main portion of the present study, participants (first- and fourth-grade children, and adults; 24 per age group) carried out a visual task while hearing lists of spoken digits and received a post-list digit recall cue only occasionally, for some lists. Under these conditions, list information presumably must be extracted from a passively held store such as auditory sensory memory. The results suggest that each individual has a core memory capacity limit that can be observed clearly in circumstances in which it cannot be supplemented by mnemonic strategies, and that the capacity limit appears to increase with age during childhood. Other, attention-demanding processes also contribute to memory for attended lists.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: General | 2003

Children's Working-Memory Processes: A Response-Timing Analysis

Nelson Cowan; John N. Towse; Zoë Hamilton; J. Scott Saults; Emily M. Elliott; Jebby F. Lacey; Matthew V. Moreno; Graham J. Hitch

Recall response durations were used to clarify processing in working-memory tasks. Experiment 1 examined childrens performance in reading span, a task in which sentences were processed and the final word of each sentence was retained for subsequent recall. Experiment 2 examined the development of listening-, counting-, and digit-span task performance. Responses were much longer in the reading-and listening-span tasks than in the other span tasks, suggesting that participants in sentence-based span tasks take time to retrieve the semantic or linguistic structure as cues to recall of the sentence-final words. Response durations in working-memory tasks helped to predict academic skill and achievement, largely separate from the contributions of the memory spans themselves. Response durations thus are important in the interpretation of span task performance.


Memory & Cognition | 2002

The irrelevant-speech effect and children: Theoretical implications of developmental change

Emily M. Elliott

The irrelevant-speech effect refers to the finding of impaired recall performance in the presence of irrelevant auditory stimuli. Two broad classes of theories exist for the effect, both allowing automatic entry of the distracting sounds into the processing system but differing in how attention is involved. As one source of evidence in the discussion of existing theories of the irrelevant-speech effect, the performance of children and adults on a visual serial recall task with irrelevant sounds (speech and tones) was examined. The magnitude of the effects of irrelevant sounds on performance decreased with age. The developmental differences were marked in the conditions with the greatest need for attentional control (words and especially changing words). The findings were interpreted with respect to current models of memory. Theories of the irrelevant-speech effect that include a role for attentional control were better suited to handle the results than those without a specified role for attention.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2010

The relationships of working memory, secondary memory, and general fluid intelligence: working memory is special.

Jill T. Shelton; Emily M. Elliott; Russell A. Matthews; B Hill; Wm. Drew Gouvier

Recent efforts have been made to elucidate the commonly observed link between working memory and reasoning ability. The results have been inconsistent, with some work suggesting that the emphasis placed on retrieval from secondary memory by working memory tests is the driving force behind this association (Mogle, Lovett, Stawski, & Sliwinski, 2008), whereas other research suggests retrieval from secondary memory is only partly responsible for the observed link between working memory and reasoning (Unsworth & Engle, 2006, 2007). In the present study, we investigated the relationship between processing speed, working memory, secondary memory, primary memory, and fluid intelligence. Although our findings show that all constructs are significantly correlated with fluid intelligence, working memory-but not secondary memory-accounts for significant unique variance in fluid intelligence. Our data support predictions made by Unsworth and Engle (2006, 2007) and suggest that the combined need for maintenance and retrieval processes present in working memory tests makes them special in their prediction of higher order cognition.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2003

List composition and the word length effect in immediate recall: a comparison of localist and globalist assumptions.

Nelson Cowan; Alan D. Baddeley; Emily M. Elliott; Jennifer Norris

Lists of short words usually are recalled better than lists of longer words in immediate recall tasks. Such word length effects might be explained bylocalist accounts, in which the length of each word in a list affects the recall of that word only, or byglobalist accounts, in which the lengths of at least some words affect the recall of other words (e.g., Baddeley, 1986). In a recent localist account, Neath and Nairne (1995) proposed that the recall of each word depends on the likelihood that features within the word are contaminated within the memory representation. We tested this by presenting not only homogeneous lists of short and long words, but also mixed lists, and by including articulatory suppression on some trials. The short-word advantage depended on the composition of the list, ruling out a strictly localist approach. There appear to be several globalist influences on recall, including distinctiveness factors as well as phonological storage and articulation.


Memory & Cognition | 2005

Coherence of the Irrelevant-Sound Effect: Individual Profiles of Short-term Memory and Susceptibility to Task-Irrelevant Materials

Emily M. Elliott; Nelson Cowan

We examined individual and developmental differences in the disruptive effects of irrelevant sounds on serial recall of printed lists. In Experiment 1, we examined adults (N = 205) receiving eight-item lists to be recalled. Although their susceptibility to disruption of recall by irrelevant sounds was only slightly related to memory span, regression analyses documented highly reliable individual differences in this susceptibility across speech and tone distractors, even with variance from span level removed. In Experiment 2, we examined adults (n = 64) and 8-year-old children (n = 63) receiving lists of a length equal to a predetermined span and one item shorter (span - 1). We again found significant relationships between measures of span and susceptibility to irrelevant sounds, although in only two of the measures. We conclude that some of the cognitive processes helpful in performing a span task may not be beneficial in the presence of irrelevant sounds.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1998

The nature of cross-modal color-word interference effects

Emily M. Elliott; Nelson Cowan; Fernando Valle-Inclan

Cowan and Barron (1987) and Cowan (1989b) reported that color-naming performance was slowed by spoken color names drawn from the same set but presented in an order unrelated to the printed colors. Although Miles, Madden, and Jones (1989) and Miles and Jones (1989) were unable to replicate this cross-modal effect, it is replicated here in two experiments with much better experimental control than before. However, the effect is shown to depend upon the relative timing of the color and word in a way that conflicts with the theoretical account that Cowan and Barron offered. While Cowan and Barron suggested that an irrelevant color word would contaminate the response set if this word occupied short-term memory when the color was about to be named, it appears that interference actually occurs only if the memory representation was formed very recently and had not been inhibited. Further implications for processing are discussed.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2000

Is There a Temporal Basis of the Word Length Effect? A Response to Service (1998):

Nelson Cowan; Lara D. Nugent; Emily M. Elliott; Tara Geer

Service (1998) carried out a study of the word length effect with Finnish pseudowords in which short and long pseudowords were identical except for the inclusion of certain phonemes differing only in pronunciation length, a manipulation that is impossible in English. She obtained an effect of phonemic complexity but little or no word duration effect per se — a discrepancy from the expectations generated by the well-known working memory model of Baddeley (1986). In the present study using English words, we controlled for phonemic complexity differences by using the same words for the short- and long-word sets, but with instructions inducing shorter or longer pronunciation of the words. We obtained substantial word duration effects. Concerns raised by Service are addressed, and we conclude that both duration and complexity are likely to contribute to the word length effect in serial recall.

Collaboration


Dive into the Emily M. Elliott's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jill T. Shelton

Louisiana State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

B Hill

University of South Alabama

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jill Talley Shelton

University of Tennessee at Chattanooga

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Katie E. Cherry

Louisiana State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Wm. Drew Gouvier

Louisiana State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge