Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Erika L. Rosenberg is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Erika L. Rosenberg.


Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | 2012

Intensive training induces longitudinal changes in meditation state related EEG oscillatory activity

Manish Saggar; Brandon G. King; Anthony P. Zanesco; Katherine A. MacLean; Stephen R. Aichele; Tonya L. Jacobs; David A. Bridwell; Phillip R. Shaver; Erika L. Rosenberg; Baljinder K. Sahdra; Emilio Ferrer; Akaysha C. Tang; George R. Mangun; B. Alan Wallace; Risto Miikkulainen; Clifford D. Saron

The capacity to focus ones attention for an extended period of time can be increased through training in contemplative practices. However, the cognitive processes engaged during meditation that support trait changes in cognition are not well characterized. We conducted a longitudinal wait-list controlled study of intensive meditation training. Retreat participants practiced focused attention (FA) meditation techniques for three months during an initial retreat. Wait-list participants later undertook formally identical training during a second retreat. Dense-array scalp-recorded electroencephalogram (EEG) data were collected during 6 min of mindfulness of breathing meditation at three assessment points during each retreat. Second-order blind source separation, along with a novel semi-automatic artifact removal tool (SMART), was used for data preprocessing. We observed replicable reductions in meditative state-related beta-band power bilaterally over anteriocentral and posterior scalp regions. In addition, individual alpha frequency (IAF) decreased across both retreats and in direct relation to the amount of meditative practice. These findings provide evidence for replicable longitudinal changes in brain oscillatory activity during meditation and increase our understanding of the cortical processes engaged during meditation that may support long-term improvements in cognition.


Review of General Psychology | 1998

Levels of analysis and the organization of affect.

Erika L. Rosenberg

This article presents a framework for the organization of affective processes, including the affective traits, moods, and emotions. Section 1 introduces the levels-of-analysis approach, defines the three levels of affect, presents criteria for ordering these levels hierarchically in terms of simple and complex temporally driven processes, and examines the interrelations among the various levels of affect, including an in-depth analysis of affective trait–emotion relationships. Section 2 offers an application of the hierarchical view to research on affect–cognition interactions, including a brief review of affect congruency effects and a discussion of the conceptual and empirical challenges to such research necessitated by consideration of the differences among the levels of affect.


Psychological Science | 2010

Intensive Meditation Training Improves Perceptual Discrimination and Sustained Attention

Katherine A. MacLean; Emilio Ferrer; Stephen R. Aichele; David A. Bridwell; Anthony P. Zanesco; Tonya L. Jacobs; Brandon G. King; Erika L. Rosenberg; Baljinder K. Sahdra; Phillip R. Shaver; B. Alan Wallace; George R. Mangun; Clifford D. Saron

The ability to focus one’s attention underlies success in many everyday tasks, but voluntary attention cannot be sustained for extended periods of time. In the laboratory, sustained-attention failure is manifest as a decline in perceptual sensitivity with increasing time on task, known as the vigilance decrement. We investigated improvements in sustained attention with training (~5 hr/day for 3 months), which consisted of meditation practice that involved sustained selective attention on a chosen stimulus (e.g., the participant’s breath). Participants were randomly assigned either to receive training first (n = 30) or to serve as waiting-list controls and receive training second (n = 30). Training produced improvements in visual discrimination that were linked to increases in perceptual sensitivity and improved vigilance during sustained visual attention. Consistent with the resource model of vigilance, these results suggest that perceptual improvements can reduce the resource demand imposed by target discrimination and thus make it easier to sustain voluntary attention.


Psychoneuroendocrinology | 2011

Intensive meditation training, immune cell telomerase activity, and psychological mediators

Tonya L. Jacobs; Elissa S. Epel; Jue Lin; Elizabeth H. Blackburn; Owen M. Wolkowitz; David A. Bridwell; Anthony P. Zanesco; Stephen R. Aichele; Baljinder K. Sahdra; Katherine A. MacLean; Brandon G. King; Phillip R. Shaver; Erika L. Rosenberg; Emilio Ferrer; B. Alan Wallace; Clifford D. Saron

BACKGROUND Telomerase activity is a predictor of long-term cellular viability, which decreases with chronic psychological distress (Epel et al., 2004). Buddhist traditions claim that meditation decreases psychological distress and promotes well-being (e.g., Dalai Lama and Cutler, 2009). Therefore, we investigated the effects of a 3-month meditation retreat on telomerase activity and two major contributors to the experience of stress: Perceived Control (associated with decreased stress) and Neuroticism (associated with increased subjective distress). We used mediation models to test whether changes in Perceived Control and Neuroticism explained meditation retreat effects on telomerase activity. In addition, we investigated whether two qualities developed by meditative practice, increased Mindfulness and Purpose in Life, accounted for retreat-related changes in the two stress-related variables and in telomerase activity. METHODS Retreat participants (n=30) meditated for ∼6 h daily for 3 months and were compared with a wait-list control group (n=30) matched for age, sex, body mass index, and prior meditation experience. Retreat participants received instruction in concentrative meditation techniques and complementary practices used to cultivate benevolent states of mind (Wallace, 2006). Psychological measures were assessed pre- and post-retreat. Peripheral blood mononuclear cell samples were collected post-retreat for telomerase activity. Because there were clear, a priori hypotheses, 1-tailed significance criteria were used throughout. RESULTS Telomerase activity was significantly greater in retreat participants than in controls at the end of the retreat (p<0.05). Increases in Perceived Control, decreases in Neuroticism, and increases in both Mindfulness and Purpose in Life were greater in the retreat group (p<0.01). Mediation analyses indicated that the effect of the retreat on telomerase was mediated by increased Perceived Control and decreased Neuroticism. In turn, changes in Perceived Control and Neuroticism were both partially mediated by increased Mindfulness and Purpose in Life. Additionally, increases in Purpose in Life directly mediated the telomerase group difference, whereas increases in Mindfulness did not. CONCLUSIONS This is the first study to link meditation and positive psychological change with telomerase activity. Although we did not measure baseline telomerase activity, the data suggest that increases in perceived control and decreases in negative affectivity contributed to an increase in telomerase activity, with implications for telomere length and immune cell longevity. Further, Purpose in Life is influenced by meditative practice and directly affects both perceived control and negative emotionality, affecting telomerase activity directly as well as indirectly.


Cognition & Emotion | 1994

Coherence between expressive and experiential systems in emotion

Erika L. Rosenberg; Paul Ekman

Abstract In order to assess the extent of coherence in emotional response systems, we examined the relationship between facial expression and self-report of emotion at multiple points in time during an affective episode. We showed subjects brief films that were selected for their ability to elicit disgust and fear, and we asked them to report on their emotions using a new reporting procedure. This procedure, called cued-review, allows subjects to rate the degree to which they experienced each of several categories of emotion for many locations over the time interval of a stimulus period. When facial expressions and reports of emotion were analysed for specific moments in film time, there was a high degree of temporal linkage and categorical agreement between facial expression and self-report, as predicted. Coherence was even stronger for more intense emotional events. This is the first evidence of linkage between facial expression and self-report of emotion on a momentary basis.


Emotion | 2011

Enhanced response inhibition during intensive meditation training predicts improvements in self-reported adaptive socioemotional functioning.

Baljinder K. Sahdra; Katherine A. MacLean; Emilio Ferrer; Phillip R. Shaver; Erika L. Rosenberg; Tonya L. Jacobs; Anthony P. Zanesco; Brandon G. King; Stephen R. Aichele; David A. Bridwell; George R. Mangun; Shiri Lavy; B. Alan Wallace; Clifford D. Saron

We examined the impact of training-induced improvements in self-regulation, operationalized in terms of response inhibition, on longitudinal changes in self-reported adaptive socioemotional functioning. Data were collected from participants undergoing 3 months of intensive meditation training in an isolated retreat setting (Retreat 1) and a wait-list control group that later underwent identical training (Retreat 2). A 32-min response inhibition task (RIT) was designed to assess sustained self-regulatory control. Adaptive functioning (AF) was operationalized as a single latent factor underlying self-report measures of anxious and avoidant attachment, mindfulness, ego resilience, empathy, the five major personality traits (extroversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience), difficulties in emotion regulation, depression, anxiety, and psychological well-being. Participants in Retreat 1 improved in RIT performance and AF over time whereas the controls did not. The control participants later also improved on both dimensions during their own retreat (Retreat 2). These improved levels of RIT performance and AF were sustained in follow-up assessments conducted approximately 5 months after the training. Longitudinal dynamic models with combined data from both retreats showed that improvement in RIT performance during training influenced the change in AF over time, which is consistent with a key claim in the Buddhist literature that enhanced capacity for self-regulation is an important precursor of changes in emotional well-being.


Motivation and Emotion | 1995

Conceptual and methodological issues in the judgment of facial expressions of emotion

Erika L. Rosenberg; Paul Ekman

In two studies, subjects judged a set of facial expressions of emotion by either providing labels of their own choice to describe the stimuli (free-choice condition), choosing a label from a list of emotion words, or choosing a story from a list of emotion stories (fixed-choice conditions). In the free-choice condition, levels of agreement between subjects on the predicted emotion categories for six basic emotions were significantly greater than chance levels, and comparable to those shown in fixed-choice studies. As predicted, there was little to no agreement on a verbal label for contempt. Agreement on contempt was greatly improved when subjects were allowed to identify the expression in terms of an antecedent event for that emotion rather than in terms of a single verbal label, a finding that could not be attributed to the methodological artifact of exclusion in a fixed-choice paradigm. These findings support two conclusions: (1) that the labels used in fixed-choice paradigms accurately reflect the verbal categories people use when free labeling facial expressions of emotion, and (2) that lexically ambiguous emotions, such as contempt, are understood in terms of their situational meanings.


BMC Neuroscience | 2010

A computational approach to understanding the longitudinal changes in cortical activity associated with intensive meditation training.

Manish Saggar; Stephen R. Aichele; Tonya L. Jacobs; Anthony P. Zanesco; David A. Bridwell; Katherine A. MacLean; Brandon G. King; Baljinder K. Sahdra; Erika L. Rosenberg; Phillip R. Shaver; Emilio Ferrer; B. Alan Wallace; George R. Mangun; Clifford D. Saron; Risto Miikkulainen

Manish Saggar and Risto Miikkulainen are with the Department of Computer Science, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA -- Stephen R Aichele, Baljinder K Sahdra, Phillip R Shaver, Emilio Ferrer, and George R Mangun are with the Department of Psychology, University of California-Davis, Davis, CA 95618, USA -- Stephen R Aichele, Tonya L Jacobs, Anthony P Zanesco, David A Bridwell, Katherine A Maclean, Brandon G King, Baljinder K Sahdra, Erika L Rosenberg, George R Mangun, Clifford D Saron are with the Center for Mind and Brain, University of California-Davis, Davis, CA 95618, USA -- David A Bridwell is with the Department of Cognitive Science, Univ. of California-Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA -- B Alan Wallace is with the Santa Barbara Institute for Consciousness Studies, Santa Barbara, CA 93130, USA -- Katherine A Maclean is with the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, JHU School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21224, USA


Health Psychology | 1998

Facial expression and the affective component of cynical hostility in male coronary heart disease patients.

Erika L. Rosenberg; Paul Ekman; James A. Blumenthal

This study describes the affective component of hostility as measured by the Cook-Medley Hostility Scale (Ho; W. Cook & D. Medley, 1954) by examining the relationship between facial expressions of emotion and Ho scores in 116 male coronary heart disease patients. Patients underwent the videotaped Type A Structured Interview, from which facial expressions were later coded using the Facial Action Coding System. They also completed the Cook-Medley Ho scale. Facial expression of the emotion of contempt was significantly related to Ho scores; anger expression was not. Also, there was a significant interaction between hostility and defensiveness, wherein low-defensive, highly hostile people showed substantially more contempt expression than others. The implications of these findings for the construct validity of Ho and for identifying clinically important subtypes of hostility are discussed.


Emotion | 2001

Linkages between facial expressions of anger and transient myocardial ischemia in men with coronary artery disease.

Erika L. Rosenberg; Paul Ekman; Wei Jiang; Michael A. Babyak; R. Edward Coleman; Michael W. Hanson; Christopher M. O'Connor; Robert A. Waugh; James A. Blumenthal

The authors examined whether facial expressions of emotion would predict changes in heart function. One hundred fifteen male patients with coronary artery disease underwent the Type A Structured Interview, during which time measures of transient myocardial ischemia (wall motion abnormality and left ventricular ejection fraction) were obtained. Facial behavior exhibited during the ischemia measurement period was videotaped and later coded by using the Facial Action Coding System (P. Ekman & W. V. Friesen, 1978). Those participants who exhibited ischemia showed significantly more anger expressions and nonenjoyment smiles than nonischemics. Cook-Medley Hostility scores did not vary with ischemic status. The findings have implications for understanding how anger and hostility differentially influence coronary heart disease risk.

Collaboration


Dive into the Erika L. Rosenberg's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Emilio Ferrer

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Katherine A. MacLean

Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge