Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Ernst Wolf is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Ernst Wolf.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1950

Dark Adaptation Level and Size of Testfield

Ernst Wolf; Michael J. Zigler

Data for dark adaptation curves were obtained for four square fields, subtending respectively 2.5, 5, 10, and 20 degrees at the eye, (a) when the subject indicated when light could just be perceived and (b) when parallel fine lines could just be detected. The levels attained in both the cone and in the rod segments of this function were found to vary when the area of the testfield exposed to the eye was changed, being higher for a smaller than for a larger testfield. This denotes that sensitivity is greater for a larger than for a smaller field of retinal stimulation. These findings indicate that the limit between so called photopic and scotopic vision is not to be regarded as fixed and definite but as functionally variable.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1954

Location of the Break in the Dark Adaptation Curve in Relation to Pre-Exposure Brightness and Pre-Exposure Time

Ernst Wolf; Michael J. Zigler

Dark adaptation curves are obtained by presenting a 2° square test field, subdivided by vertical bars, 6° in the lateral parafovea. When pre-exposure time is constant (10 minutes), and pre-exposure brightness is progressively decreased, the cone plateau gradually drops to lower brightness levels, while the final rod level remains constant. However, when pre-exposure brightness is constant (1510 millilamberts), and pre-exposure time is varied, the cone plateau and the final rod level remain unchanged. The break in the duplex curve shifts to earlier times with decreasing pre-exposure brightness as well as with decreasing preexposure time. Linear relationships exist between time of appearance of the break (minutes) and log preexposure brightness, or log pre-exposure time. The slope constants are different, however, indicating that pre-exposure brightness and pre-exposure time are not reciprocally interchangeable as far as the location of the break is concerned.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1955

Course of dark adaptation under various conditions of pre-exposure and testing.

Ernst Wolf; Michael J. Zigler

Dark adaptation curves are obtained by presenting a 2° square testfield 6° below the center of the fovea for both eyes tested separately and for both eyes tested alternately. When the pre-exposure luminances for the two eyes differ, the independently recorded dark adaptation curves for the two eyes show differences in correspondence with the difference in radiant energy delivered to the eyes during pre-exposure. However, if both eyes are pre-exposed simultaneously to their respective luminances, and are alternately tested, the dark adaptation curves are not identical with those previously found. They move closer together, or coincide, i.e., the differences in threshold sensitivity become smaller, or disappear. When pre-exposure of the two eyes differs merely in admitting the near ultraviolet (285–400 mμ) to one eye, and screening it from the other, the curves independently recorded for the two eyes have different threshold levels, but in alternate testing no difference is found. It is also shown that an influence of the near ultraviolet upon visual thresholds is readily found when white and blue testlights are employed; it is barely evident when a green testlight is used and is not found with a red testlight.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1963

Sensitivity of the Retinal Area in One Eye Corresponding to the Blind Spot in the Other Eye

Ernst Wolf; Jean S. Gardiner

Threshold sensitivity was studied in six observers on one vertical and three horizontal lines running through an area in the nasal visual field of one eye which corresponds to the blind-spot region of the other eye. The test fields were squares of 1° angular subtense on a side and were presented for 0.04 sec. Thresholds were found to be lower in this region than at retinal points immediately outside, yielding a mean increase in sensitivity of approximately 0.25 log unit. The findings are discussed in regard to density and innervation of retinal elements, cortical representation, and the possibility of compensation for lack of sensitivity in the blind spot in order to maintain uniform brightness in an integrated binocular field of vision.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1963

Effects of Uniocular and Binocular Excitation of the Peripheral Retina with Test Fields of Various Shapes on Binocular Summation

Ernst Wolf; Michael J. Zigler

Square test fields subtending a visual angle of 1° on a side were presented at 28 positions in the field of vision on a circle with a radius of 10° around fixation. Visual thresholds for the right eye, the left eye, and for both eyes were determined on two observers. In the lateral periphery the binocular thresholds are lower than the monocular thresholds (binocular summation); near the vertical meridian above and below fixation, however, binocular sensitivity is not greater than monocular sensitivity. When, instead of squares, rectangular test fields of 12°×2° and 14°×4° are presented in horizontal or vertical position, it is found that on the median line no binocular summation occurs when the test fields are presented vertically, but summation prevails when the test fields are presented horizontally. It appears that the amount of overlap of a test field into the lateral periphery and the conveying of impulses from both retinas to the same cerebral hemisphere is important for binocular summation.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1951

Dark Adaptation Level and Duration of Testflash

Ernst Wolf; Michael J. Zigler

Using a testfield subtending a visual angle of 5 degrees, dark adaptation functions were obtained with six test-exposures (1.0, 0.5, 0.1, 0.04, 0.02, and 0.01 sec) both from the central retina, and from the temporal parafovea. The functions for both retinal locations were found to vary systematically in relation to duration of testflash. With increasing test-exposure time, the final cone levels as well as the final rod levels are at lower threshold brightnesses. The sensitivity for a given test-exposure time is greater in the central retina than in the parafovea for the photopic range of vision, whereas the sensitivity for the scotopic range is greater in the parafovea than in the central retina.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1951

The Influence of Surround Brightness and Short Wave Components of Radiation on Dark Adaptation

Michael J. Zigler; Ernst Wolf; Esther Shores King

Dark adaptation functions were determined by presenting a 2° square test field in the temporal parafovea, or a 1.3° field in the fovea, against backgrounds or surround luminances varying in brightness from complete darkness to 40 millilamberts. The test field was composed of parallel, equally spaced dark and light vertical bars, and presented in the form of flashes of 0.04 sec. As surround luminance is systematically increased the total extent of adaptation, the relative participation of rods and cones, and the transition from cone to rod function vary as a function of background luminance. Pre-exposure to a light source emitting radiation from 2850 to 4100 angstroms in addition to white light has an inhibitory effect on the sensitivity of the retinal receptors which is evident in higher excitation thresholds as compared with those obtained when the ultraviolet is screened from the pre-exposure source. The ultraviolet action which previously was only shown for the rods is also found in foveal and parafoveal cones, when, owing to the presence of a surround luminance, the rods are prevented from entering into action, and the range of adaptation is limited to levels above threshold for cone excitation.


Journal of the Optical Society of America | 1965

Excitation of the Peripheral Retina with Coincident and Disparate Test Fields

Ernst Wolf; Michael J. Zigler

Rectangular test fields 12°×2°, with long dimension horizontal and vertical, were presented 10° from fixation on the horizontal and vertical meridian of the visual field. Threshold luminances were determined for each eye singly, and for both eyes when the retinal images were coincident or disparate. In the lateral visual field, binocular thresholds for coincident images were lower than when they were disparate. On the vertical meridian, binocular summation of coincident images is insignificant or totally absent. If, however, in this retinal location the images are disparate and the image in one eye falls to one side of the median line, summation occurs. It is thought that binocular summation is a function of the transmission of impulses from both eyes to the same cortical area of the same cerebral hemisphere.


Archives of Ophthalmology | 1974

Intraocular Light Scattering: Theory and Clinical Application.

Ernst Wolf

This 121-page book presents, in five chapters, the essentials on physical theory, structural peculiarities of normal and abnormal transparent tissues of the eye, the effects of changes in transmissiveness of the dioptric media on the clarity of retinal images, measurements and evaluation of vision loss in relation to structural changes, and means of improving vision by methods presently available and being developed. The first chapter is concerned with the theory of light scattering. In a turbid medium a light beam loses energy in proportion to the density of scattering particles. When density is less than half the wavelength of light, no scatter occurs. In the normal transparent tissue of the eye the elements of cellular structures are so tightly packed that they are transparent. Electron micrographs of normal and edmatous corneal stroma serve as illustrations. In the lens cataract formation is responsible for light scattering leading to the sensation of


Archives of Ophthalmology | 1971

Color Blindness: An Evolutionary Approach.

Ernst Wolf

The book offers a description of basic physical properties of light, color, the pigments, and their role in visual processes; it discusses color vision theories, anomalies, their diagnosis, and classification. In view of the size of the book, the basic physiological facts are treated not in too much detail but contain a fair and up-to-date introduction to the understanding of color vision and methods of color vision testing. The most important and valuable chapters make up the second half of the book. The author discusses the causes, origin, geographic distribution, and evolution of color deficiencies. Color vision and color blindness in man has a long evolutionary history which can be traced historically and geographically as for instance the distribution of color blindness in Africa and South America where deficiencies prevail in coastal regions while they are absent in the center of the continents. In the color deficient, lack of sensitivity

Collaboration


Dive into the Ernst Wolf's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Joel Newman

Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge