Erwin Nemeth
Max Planck Society
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Erwin Nemeth.
Animal Behaviour | 2009
Erwin Nemeth; Henrik Brumm
When animals colonize cities they often have to adapt their physiology, life history and behaviour to the novel environment. Songbirds rely on acoustic communication for reproduction, and recent studies indicate that songs vary between urban and nonurban habitats. In cities, birds sing louder or use higher frequencies compared to their conspecifics in forests. These habitat-specific differences in song have been interpreted as an adaptation of the city birds to mitigate acoustic masking by low-frequency traffic noise. We compared the songs of blackbirds, Turdus merula, from the city centre of Vienna and the Vienna Woods and found that forest birds sang at lower frequencies and with longer intervals between songs. This difference in song pitch might reflect an adaptation to urban ambient noise. However, the song divergence could also be the result of more intense vocal interaction in the more densely populated city areas or a side-effect of physiological adaptation to urban habitats. We emphasize the need for experimental studies in blackbirds, but also in other species, to clarify a possible causal link between urban acoustics and song characteristics of city birds.
The American Naturalist | 2010
Erwin Nemeth; Henrik Brumm
In cities with intense low‐frequency traffic noise, birds have been observed to sing louder and at a higher pitch. Several studies argue that higher song pitch is an adaptation to reduce masking from noise, and it has even been suggested that the song divergence between urban and nonurban songs might lead to reproductive isolation. Here we present models of signal transmission to compare the benefits of raised song amplitude and song pitch in terms of sound transmission. We chose two bird species that sing with higher pitch in urban areas, the great tit (Parus major) and the blackbird (Turdus merula). For both species, we calculated communication distances in response to different levels of urban noise and in their natural forest habitats. We found that an increase in vocal pitch increased communication distance only marginally. In contrast, vocal amplitude adjustments had a strong and significantly larger effect. Our results indicate that frequency changes of urban songs are not very effective in mitigating masking from traffic noise. Increased song pitch might not be an adaptation to reduce signal masking but a physiological side effect of singing at high amplitudes or an epiphenomenon of urbanization that is not related to signal transmission.
Animal Behaviour | 2012
Sue Anne Zollinger; Jeffrey Podos; Erwin Nemeth; Franz Goller; Henrik Brumm
A growing number of studies ask whether and how bird songs vary between areas with low versus high levels of anthropogenic noise. Across numerous species, birds are seen to sing at higher frequencies in urban versus rural populations, presumably because of selection for higher-pitched songs in the face of low-frequency urban noise, or because birds can avoid masking directly by shifting to higher-frequency sounds (Fernandez-Juricic et al. 2005; Slabbekoorn & den Boer-Visser 2006; Nemeth & Brumm 2009; Gross et al. 2010; Potvin et al. 2010). In addition to changing song frequency, birds are also reported to respond to increased background noise by singing at higher amplitudes (Brumm & Zollinger 2011). Nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos, for example, sing with a higher sound pressure level in areas with intense traffic noise as compared to quieter locations (Brumm 2004). While frequencyand amplitude-based responses to ambient noise are often considered independently, the twomight also vary in tandem
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B : Biological Sciences | 2013
Erwin Nemeth; Nadia Pieretti; Sue Anne Zollinger; Nicole Geberzahn; Jesko Partecke; Ana Catarina Miranda; Henrik Brumm
When animals live in cities, they have to adjust their behaviour and life histories to novel environments. Noise pollution puts a severe constraint on vocal communication by interfering with the detection of acoustic signals. Recent studies show that city birds sing higher-frequency songs than their conspecifics in non-urban habitats. This has been interpreted as an adaptation to counteract masking by traffic noise. However, this notion is debated, for the observed frequency shifts seem to be less efficient at mitigating noise than singing louder, and it has been suggested that city birds might use particularly high-frequency song elements because they can be produced at higher amplitudes. Here, we present the first phonetogram for a songbird, which shows that frequency and amplitude are strongly positively correlated in the common blackbird (Turdus merula), a successful urban colonizer. Moreover, city blackbirds preferentially sang higher-frequency elements that can be produced at higher intensities and, at the same time, happen to be less masked in low-frequency traffic noise.
Frontiers in Zoology | 2014
Petra Sumasgutner; Erwin Nemeth; Graham Tebb; Harald W. Krenn; Anita Gamauf
IntroductionUrbanization is a global phenomenon that is encroaching on natural habitats and decreasing biodiversity, although it is creating new habitats for some species. The Eurasian kestrel (Falco tinnunculus) is frequently associated with urbanized landscapes but it is unclear what lies behind the high densities of kestrels in the urban environment.ResultsOccupied nest sites in the city of Vienna, Austria were investigated along a gradient of urbanization (percentage of land covered by buildings or used by traffic). Field surveys determined the abundance of potential prey (birds and rodents) and the results were compared to the birds’ diets. A number of breeding parameters were recorded over the course of three years. The majority of kestrels breed in semi-natural cavities in historic buildings. Nearest neighbour distances (NND) were smallest and reproductive success lowest in the city centre. Abundance of potential prey was not found to relate to the degree of urbanization but there was a significant shift in the birds’ diets from a heavy reliance on rodents in the outskirts of the city to feeding more on small birds in the centre. The use of urban habitats was associated with higher nest failure, partly associated with predation and nest desertion, and with significantly lower hatching rates and smaller fledged broods.ConclusionsHigh breeding densities in urban habitats do not necessarily correlate with high habitat quality. The high density of kestrel nests in the city centre is probably due to the ready availability of breeding cavities. Highly urbanized areas in Vienna are associated with unexpected costs for the city dwelling-raptor, in terms both of prey availability and of reproductive success. The kestrel appears to be exploiting the urban environment but given the poor reproductive performance of urban kestrels it is likely that the species is falling into an ecological trap.
Oryx | 2012
Michael Dvorak; Birgit Fessl; Erwin Nemeth; Sonia Kleindorfer; Sabine Tebbich
Population monitoring is a vital tool for conservation management and for testing hypotheses about population trends in changing environments. Darwin’s finches on Santa Cruz Island in the Galapagos archipelago have experienced habitat alteration because of human activity, introduced predators, parasites and disease. We used point counts to conduct systematic quantitative surveys of Darwin’s finches and other land birds between 1997 and 2010. The temporal analysis revealed that six of the nine species investigated declined significantly and that this decline was most pronounced at higher elevations in humid native forest and agricultural areas; the highland areas have been most affected by introduced species or direct human impact. Five of the six declining species are insectivorous, which suggests that changes in insect abundance or insect availability are a critical factor in the declines. Further study is required to test this idea. Other factors including habitat alteration and introduced parasites or pathogens may be contributing to the observed declines.
The American Naturalist | 2012
Erwin Nemeth; Sue Anne Zollinger; Henrik Brumm
Effect Sizes and the Integrative Understanding of Urban Bird Song (A Reply to Slabbekoornet al.)Author(s): Erwin Nemeth, Sue Anne Zollinger, and Henrik BrummReviewed work(s):Source: The American Naturalist, Vol. 180, No. 1 (July 2012), pp. 146-152Published by: The University of Chicago Press for The American Society of NaturalistsStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/665994 .Accessed: 08/06/2012 07:18
Ecology and Evolution | 2016
Davide M. Dominoni; Stefan Greif; Erwin Nemeth; Henrik Brumm
Abstract Anthropogenic noise is of increasing concern to biologists and medical scientists. Its detrimental effects on human health have been well studied, with the high noise levels from air traffic being of particular concern. However, less is known about the effects of airport noise pollution on signal masking in wild animals. Here, we report a relationship between aircraft noise and two major features of the singing behavior of birds. We found that five of ten songbird species began singing significantly earlier in the morning in the vicinity of a major European airport than their conspecifics at a quieter control site. As birds at both sites started singing before the onset of air traffic in the morning, this suggests that the birds in the vicinity of the airport advanced their activity to gain more time for unimpaired singing before the massive plane noise set in. In addition, we found that during the day, chaffinches avoided singing during airplane takeoffs, but only when the noise exceeded a certain threshold, further suggesting that the massive noise caused by the airport can impair acoustic communication in birds. Overall, our study indicates that birds may be adjusting their mating signals and time budgets in response to aircraft noise.
Animal Behaviour | 2011
Henrik Brumm; Kathryn A. Robertson; Erwin Nemeth
Directional communication, in other words the use of body orientation to increase transmission of directional signals, has been widely neglected in studies of animal communication. Here we propose that the experimental analysis of singing direction in birds can provide insights into the function of song. We studied three populations of sedge warblers, Acrocephalus schoenobaenus, in Austria, Britain and Germany. In this species, song is thought to be primarily a mate attraction signal, as males cease to sing once they are paired. Using behavioural observation during playback experiments, we found that males approached a loudspeaker simulating a rival male and also oriented themselves towards the loudspeaker while singing. This finding shows that sedge warbler songs are also directed at rival males and that the dual functions of mate attraction and territory defence were only hidden from the human observer because singing ceases after pairing. We argue that the study of directed communication is an additional tool in addressing functional questions, and that animal orientation should be taken into account when conducting investigations of signalling behaviour.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences | 2017
Sue Anne Zollinger; P.J.B. Slater; Erwin Nemeth; Henrich Brumm
It has been observed in many songbird species that populations in noisy urban areas sing with a higher minimum frequency than do matched populations in quieter, less developed areas. However, why and how this divergence occurs is not yet understood. We experimentally tested whether chronic noise exposure during vocal learning results in songs with higher minimum frequencies in great tits (Parus major), the first species for which a correlation between anthropogenic noise and song frequency was observed. We also tested vocal plasticity of adult great tits in response to changing background noise levels by measuring song frequency and amplitude as we changed noise conditions. We show that noise exposure during ontogeny did not result in songs with higher minimum frequencies. In addition, we found that adult birds did not make any frequency or song usage adjustments when their background noise conditions were changed after song crystallization. These results challenge the common view of vocal adjustments by city birds, as they suggest that either noise itself is not the causal force driving the divergence of song frequency between urban and forest populations, or that noise induces population-wide changes over a time scale of several generations rather than causing changes in individual behaviour.