Fabien Cignetti
Aix-Marseille University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Fabien Cignetti.
Annals of Biomedical Engineering | 2012
Fabien Cignetti; Leslie Decker; Nicholas Stergiou
The Wolf’s (W-algorithm) and Rosenstein’s (R-algorithm) algorithms have been used to quantify local dynamic stability (largest Lyapunov exponent, λ1) in gait, with prevalence of the latter one that is considered more suitable for small data sets. However, such a claim has never been investigated. To address it, the λ1 of the Lorenz attractor was estimated using small data sets and varied delays and embedding dimensions. Overall, the λ1 estimates from the R-algorithm got closer to the theoretical exponent than those from the W-algorithm. The W-algorithm also overestimated λ1 while the R-algorithm underestimated it, overlooking the attractor convergences and divergences, respectively. Local dynamic stability was then examined from 1-, 2- and 3-min long gait time series of younger (YA) and older adults (OA). The OA were found more locally unstable than the YA regardless of time series length with the W-algorithm but only for the longest time series with the R-algorithm. The lack of sensitivity to capture age-related decline in local dynamic stability from shorter time series is proposed to result from a drawback of the R-algorithm that overlooks the expansion of the attractor trajectories. The W-algorithm is advocated for use when examining local dynamic stability with small gait data sets.
Neurophysiologie Clinique-clinical Neurophysiology | 2014
Christine Assaiante; F. Barlaam; Fabien Cignetti; Marianne Vaugoyeau
In order to perceive and act in its environment, the individuals body and its interactions with the sensory and social environment are represented in the brain. This internal representation of the moving body segments is labeled the body schema. Throughout life, body schema develops based on the sensory information used by the moving body and by its interactions with the environment including other people. Internal representations including body schema and representations of the outside world develop with learning and actions throughout ontogenesis and are constantly updated based on different sensory inputs. The aim of this review is to present some concepts and experimental data about body schema, internal representations and updating process during childhood and adolescence, as obtained using a neurosensory approach. From our developmental studies, it was possible to explore the slow maturation of the sensorimotor representations by examining the anticipatory control. By manipulating proprioceptive and visual information, which are at the heart of the construction of body schema, we wished to highlight notable differences between adolescents and young adults on both a postural and perceptual level, which confirms the late maturation of multisensory integration for central motor control.
PLOS ONE | 2012
Leslie Decker; Fabien Cignetti; Jane F. Potter; Stephanie A. Studenski; Nicholas Stergiou
Motor abundance allows individuals to perform any task reliably while being variable in movements particulars. The study investigated age-related differences in this feature when young adults (YA) and older adults (OA) performed challenging tasks, namely treadmill walking alone and while performing a cognitive task. A goal function for treadmill walking was first defined, i.e., maintain constant speed at each step, which led to a goal equivalent manifold (GEM) containing all combinations of step time and step length that equally satisfied the function. Given the GEM, amounts of goal-equivalent and non-goal-equivalent variability were afterwards determined and used to define an index providing information about the set of effective motor solutions relative to the GEM. The set was limited in OA compared to YA in treadmill walking alone, indicating that OA made less flexible use of motor abundance than YA. However, this differentiation between YA and OA disappeared when concurrently performing the cognitive task. It is proposed that OA might have benefited from cognitive compensation.
Human Brain Mapping | 2014
Fabien Cignetti; Marianne Vaugoyeau; Bruno Nazarian; Muriel Roth; Jean-Luc Anton; Christine Assaiante
The feeling of illusory movement is considered important in the study of human behavior because it is deeply related to motor consciousness. However, the neural basis underlying the illusion of movement remains to be understood. Following optimal vibratory stimulation of muscle tendon, certain subjects experience illusory movements while others do not. In the present fMRI study, we sought to uncover the neural basis of illusory movement awareness by contrasting a posteriori these two types of subjects. Examining fMRI data using leave‐one‐subject‐out general linear models and region of interest analyses, we found that a non‐limb‐specific associative network, including the opercular part of the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right inferior parietal lobule, was more active in subjects with illusions. On the other hand, levels of activation in other brain areas involved in kinaesthetic processing were rather similar between the two subsamples of subjects. These results suggest that activation of the right inferior frontoparietal areas, once passed a certain threshold, forms the basis of illusory movements. This is consistent with the global neuronal workspace hypothesis that associates conscious processing with surges of frontoparietal activity. Hum Brain Mapp 35:5166–5178, 2014.
PLOS ONE | 2013
Fabien Cignetti; Milan Zedka; Marianne Vaugoyeau; Christine Assaiante
Although there is suggestive evidence that a link exists between independent walking and the ability to establish anticipatory strategy to stabilize posture, the extent to which this skill facilitates the development of anticipatory postural control remains largely unknown. Here, we examined the role of independent walking on the infants’ ability to anticipate predictable external perturbations. Non-walking infants, walking infants and adults were sitting on a platform that produced continuous rotation in the frontal plane. Surface electromyography (EMG) of neck and lower back muscles and the positions of markers located on the platform, the upper body and the head were recorded. Results from cross-correlation analysis between rectified and filtered EMGs and platform movement indicated that although muscle activation already occurred before platform movement in non-walking infants, only walking infants demonstrated an adult-like ability for anticipation. Moreover, results from further cross-correlation analysis between segmental angular displacement and platform movement together with measures of balance control at the end-points of rotation of the platform evidenced two sorts of behaviour. The adults behaved as a non-rigid non-inverted pendulum, rather stabilizing head in space, while both the walking and non-walking infants followed the platform, behaving as a rigid inverted pendulum. These results suggest that the acquisition of independent walking plays a role in the development of anticipatory postural control, likely improving the internal model for the sensorimotor control of posture. However, despite such improvement, integrating the dynamics of an external object, here the platform, within the model to maintain balance still remains challenging in infants.
Neuroscience | 2013
Fabien Cignetti; P.-Y. Chabeauti; Heidi Sveistrup; Marianne Vaugoyeau; Christine Assaiante
The objective of this study was to investigate the updating process of internal models of action in children and young adults, through the postural and motor strategies adopted in simple tasks, namely sit-to-stand (STS) and back-to-sit (BTS). To this end, 11 healthy children from 7 to 10years (latest stage of childhood) and 12 healthy adults participated in the experiment. The STS and BTS tasks were performed with horizontal support surface and support surface tilted 10° to the right or forward in order to investigate the immediate adaptation of the internal representations of the movement. Movement variables that included the durations of STS and BTS and the amplitudes of the trunk movement were computed. Postural characteristics were also considered during the transition from STS to BTS, including the trunk orientation and the head stabilization strategies. Despite certain similarities with adults, especially in terms of the asymmetry of the performance times for the two tasks (STS vs. BTS) and the partial movement adaptations, the children were less able than adults to adapt both postural and movement controls to the new support conditions. Thus, it appears that the updating of internal models of action is a process that matures slowly throughout ontogenesis.
Gait & Posture | 2013
Leslie Decker; Fabien Cignetti; Nicholas Stergiou
Humans apply a minimum intervention principle to regulate treadmill walking, rapidly correcting fluctuations in the task-relevant variable (step speed: SS) while ignoring fluctuations in the task-irrelevant variables (step time: ST; step length: SL). We examined whether the regulation of fluctuations in SS and not in ST and SL depends on high-level, executive function, processes. Young adults walked on a treadmill without a cognitive requirement and while performing the cognitive task of dichotic listening. SS fluctuations became less anti-persistent when performing dichotic listening, meaning that taxing executive function impaired the ability to rapidly correct speed deviations on subsequent steps. Conversely, performing dichotic listening had no effect on SL and ST persistent fluctuations. Findings suggest that high-level brain processes are involved only in regulating gait task-relevant variables.
Cerebral Cortex | 2016
Fabien Cignetti; Aurelie Fontan; Jasmine C. Menant; Bruno Nazarian; Jean Luc Anton; Marianne Vaugoyeau; Christine Assaiante
Abstract Proprioceptive processing is important for appropriate motor control, providing error‐feedback and internal representation of movement for adjusting the motor command. Although proprioceptive functioning improves during childhood and adolescence, we still have few clues about how the proprioceptive brain network develops. Here, we investigated developmental changes in the functional organization of this network in early adolescents (n = 18, 12 ± 1 years), late adolescents (n = 18, 15 ± 1), and young adults (n = 18, 32 ± 4), by examining task‐evoked univariate activity and patterns of functional connectivity (FC) associated with seeds placed in cortical (supramarginal gyrus) and subcortical (dorsal rostral putamen) regions. We found that although the network is already well established in early adolescence both in terms of topology and functioning principles (e.g., long‐distance communication and economy in wiring cost), it is still undergoing refinement during adolescence, including a shift from diffuse to focal FC and a decreased FC strength. This developmental effect was particularly pronounced for fronto‐striatal connections. Furthermore, changes in FC features continued beyond adolescence, although to a much lower extent. Altogether, these findings point to a protracted developmental time course for the proprioceptive network, which breaks with the relatively early functional maturation often associated with sensorimotor networks.
Annals of Biomedical Engineering | 2012
Fabien Cignetti; Leslie Decker; Nicholas Stergiou
Assessing gait stability using the Largest Lyapunov Exponent (k1) has become popular, especially because it may be a key measure in evaluating gait abnormalities in patient populations. However, clinical settings usually involve having small gait data sets and accurate determination of k1 estimates from such sets is difficult. In an effort to address this issue, Cignetti et al. recently identified that k1 estimates using the algorithm of Wolf et al. (W-algorithm) were more sensitive than those using the algorithm of Rosenstein et al. (R-algorithm) in order to capture age-related decline in gait stability from small data sets. Thus, they advocated the use of the former algorithm. Some concerns about the study were expressed afterwards by Bruijn et al. and we welcome the opportunity to discuss them in the present letter. Bruijn et al. expressed four concerns about the validity of the methods used by Cignetti et al. that could have biased the results. First, they indicate that although speed difference between young adults (YA) and older adults (OA) was not significant, it does not exclude speed as a confounder of the aging effect on gait stability. Although we agree that a perfect matching of YA and OA with respect to speed would definitely avoid confounding, such matching is highly unlikely as YA walk usually faster than OA. Accordingly, matching statistically the two groups in terms of average speed appears to be the best compromise between ecological validity and methodological validity. However, a mean to further avoid the confounding of speed on k1 is to evaluate group difference by using analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) instead of using analyses of variance (ANOVAs), thus controlling for speed effect. As reported in Table 1, results from ANCOVAs run on the data sets of Cignetti et al. confirmed previous results obtained using ANOVAs. In particular, with respect to the main effect of age, k1 remained significantly larger in OA as compared to YA regardless the size of the data set (i.e., 3600, 7200, and 10,800 data points) when using the W-algorithm. Such result was obtained only for the largest data set (i.e., 10,800 data points) when using the R-algorithm. Therefore, the difference in k1 between YA and OA reported by Cignetti et al. is not biased by the inter-group difference in walking speed. Second, Bruijn et al. argued that the time series of YA might have counted more strides than those of OA due to shorter stride time, increasing artificially k1. However, the stride time was the same in YA and OA with mean ± standard error values of 1.27 ± 0.03 and 1.26 ± 0.05 s, respectively (Table 2). These data are in agreement with previous studies that reported similar values of stride time in both YA and OA populations. Accordingly, k1 exponents were estimated in the study of Cignetti et al. from a similar number of strides for both groups. Specifically, the time series with 3600 data points contained 47 strides in both YA and OA, the time series with 7200 data points contained 94 strides in YA and 95 strides in OA, and the time series with 10,800 data points contained 141 strides in YA and 143 strides in OA (Table 2). Hence, Bruijn et al. were mistaken in assuming that an intergroup difference in stride time could have biased the difference in k1 between YA and OA in Cignetti et al.’s study. A third concern expressed by Bruijn et al., closely related to the previous one, relates with the fact that Cignetti et al. did not normalize time using average stride time when estimating k1 with the W-algorithm, Address correspondence to Fabien Cignetti, Nebraska Biomechanics Core Facility, University of Nebraska at Omaha, 6001 Dodge Street, Omaha, NE 68182-0216, USA. Electronic mail: fabien. [email protected] Annals of Biomedical Engineering, Vol. 40, No. 12, December 2012 ( 2012) pp. 2507–2509 DOI: 10.1007/s10439-012-0665-6
GeroScience | 2017
Maroua Belghali; Nathalie Chastan; Fabien Cignetti; Damien Davenne; Leslie M. Decker
Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases are age-related progressive neurodegenerative diseases of increasing prevalence worldwide. In the absence of curative therapy, current research is interested in prevention, by identifying subtle signs of early-stage neurodegeneration. Today, the field of behavioral neuroscience has emerged as one of the most promising areas of research on this topic. Recently, it has been shown that the exacerbation of gait disorders under dual-task conditions (i.e., simultaneous performance of cognitive and motor tasks) could be a characteristic feature of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. The cognitive-motor dual-task paradigm during walking allows to assess whether (i) executive attention is abnormally impaired in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease or (ii) compensation strategies are used in order to preserve gait function when the basal ganglia system is altered in prodromal Parkinson’s disease. This review aims at (i) identifying patterns of dual-task-related gait changes that are specific to Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases, respectively, (ii) demonstrating that these changes could potentially be used as prediagnostic markers for disease onset, (iii) reviewing pros and cons of existing dual-task studies, and (iv) proposing future directions for clinical research.