Fabien Trémeau
New York University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Fabien Trémeau.
Clinical Psychology Review | 2011
Julie Messinger; Fabien Trémeau; Daniel Antonius; Erika Mendelsohn; Vasthie Prudent; Arielle D. Stanford; Dolores Malaspina
The DSM-5 formulation presents an opportunity to refine the negative symptom assessments that are crucial for a schizophrenia diagnosis. This review traces the history of negative symptom constructs in neuropsychiatry from their earliest conceptualizations in the 19th century. It presents the relevant literature for distinguishing between different types of negative symptoms. Although a National Institute of Mental Health consensus initiative proposed that there are five separate negative symptom domains, our review of the individual items demonstrates no more than three negative symptom domains. Indeed, numerous factor analyses of separate negative symptom scales routinely identify only two domains: 1) expressive deficits, which include affective, linguistic and paralinguistic expressions, and 2) avolition for daily life and social activities. We propose that a focus on expressive deficits and avolition will be of optimum utility for diagnosis, treatment-considerations, and research purposes compared to other negative symptom constructs. We recommend that these two domains should be assessed as separate dimensions in the DSM-5 criteria.
Schizophrenia Research | 2009
Fabien Trémeau; Daniel Antonius; John T. Cacioppo; Rachel Ziwich; Maria Jalbrzikowski; Erica Saccente; Gail Silipo; Pamela D. Butler; Daniel C. Javitt
BACKGROUND Ambivalence and anhedonia have long been identified as schizophrenic symptoms. However, ambivalence has rarely been studied, and in most evocative studies, schizophrenia participants are not anhedonic. Affective neurosciences posit two evaluative systems (one for Positivity and one for Negativity), the coactivation of which produces ambivalence, and point to two asymmetries in affective processing: Positivity Offset (which measures our capacity to explore the environment) and Negativity Bias (a measure of reactivity to intense threat). These characteristics have not received much attention in schizophrenia research. METHODS Sixty-four individuals with schizophrenia and 32 non-patient control participants completed an evocative emotional task with pictures, sounds and words of various valences and intensities. Following each presentation, participants rated the level of pleasantness, unpleasantness, and arousal elicited by the stimulus. Finally, participants completed questionnaires on anhedonia, and practical life skills were assessed. RESULTS Schizophrenia participants showed higher levels of ambivalence, greater arousal, greater Positivity Offset, and non-significantly different hedonic capacities and Negativity Bias. Ambivalence to positive stimuli significantly correlated with duration of illness, current level of psychopathology, anhedonia questionnaires and practical life skills. Schizophrenia patients with negative symptoms did not differ from patients without negative symptoms on computer tasks. CONCLUSIONS Ambivalence is greater in schizophrenia, and can be understood as a de-differentiation of the activation of the two evaluative systems. Ambivalence to positive stimuli, which may reflect early-stage affective processing is associated with impairments in higher-level emotional processes and in everyday functioning. Future studies should clarify the status of anhedonia in schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2000
Humberto Correa; Fabrice Duval; Marie-Claude Mokrani; P. Bailey; Fabien Trémeau; Luc Staner; Than-Son Diep; Yann Hode; Marc-Antoine Crocq; Jean-Paul Macher
Previous studies of the prolactin response to D-fenfluramine in depressed patients have yielded inconsistent results. This may be because they did not address the question of suicidality. We carried out this study to test the hypothesis that lower prolactin response to D-fenfluramine is more closely associated with suicidal behavior than with depression itself. A D-fenfluramine test was performed in a sample of 18 healthy control subjects and in 85 drug-free inpatients with a DSM-III-R diagnosis of major depressive episode (49 with a history of suicide attempt, 36 without). Depressed inpatients with a history of suicide attempt showed a significantly lower prolactin response to D-fenfluramine compared to depressed inpatients without such a history and compared to control subjects. Healthy control subjects and depressed inpatients without a history of suicide attempt showed comparable levels of prolactin after D-fenfluramine. Time elapsed since suicide attempt did not influence prolactin level (baseline or post-stimulation). Results show that in our depressed drug-free inpatient sample, prolactin response to D-fenfluramine seems to be a marker of suicidality, but not of depression itself. We suggest that it is a trait marker of suicidality.
Schizophrenia Research | 2002
Humberto Correa; Fabrice Duval; Marie-Claude Mokrani; P. Bailey; Fabien Trémeau; Luc Staner; Than-Son Diep; Marc-Antoine Crocq; Jean-Paul Macher
Abstract Recent studies suggest that altered serotonergic (5-HT) function, as assessed by lower prolactin (PRL) response to fenfluramine (FEN), a specific 5-HT releaser and uptake inhibitor, is associated with suicidal behavior in either depressed and personality disordered patients. The purpose of this study was to investigate, in schizophrenic patients, the relationship between suicidal behavior and PRL response to d -fenfluramine ( d -FEN). A d -FEN test was performed in 18 healthy controls and 33 drug-free DSM-IV schizophrenic patients (12 with a history of suicide attempts, 21 without it). Schizophrenic patients with a history of suicide attempts showed a lower PRL response to d -FEN (ΔPRL) compared to schizophrenic patients without such history ( P P d -FEN is a marker of suicidality also in schizophrenia, supporting the hypothesis that a dysfunction in serotonergic function is associated with suicidal behavior regardless of the psychiatric diagnosis.
Therapeutic Drug Monitoring | 1998
Pascal Kintz; Antoine Tracqui; Catherine Marzullo; Angélina Darreye; Fabien Trémeau; Philippe Greth; Bertrand Ludes
In recent years, remarkable advances in sensitive analytical techniques have enabled the analysis of drugs in unconventional samples, such as sweat. In a study conducted during a methadone maintenance program, PharmChek sweat patches were applied to 20 subjects. The subjects were orally administered methadone in 1 dosage/day, and doses ranged from 80 to 100 mg. The sweat patch was applied 10 minutes before administration and removed 72 hours later just before a new administration of methadone. The absorbent pad was stored at -20 degrees C until analysis in plastic tubes. Methadone was extracted in 5 ml methanol in presence of 200 ng of racemic methadone-d3, used as internal standard. After a 30-minute agitation, the methanol solution was evaporated to dryness. Enantioselective separation of methadone was obtained using an alpha-1-acid glycoprotein column (100 x 4 mm ID) and liquid chromatography/ion spray-mass spectrometry. In all 20 specimens obtained from subjects under racemic methadone treatment, R- (the active form) and S-enantiomers of methadone were identified with the following concentrations: 26 to 1118 ng/patch for R-methadone and 28 to 1114 ng/patch for S-methadone. The ratio between R- and S-methadone was in the range of 0.72 to 2.66 and was higher than 1.00 in 15 samples. No correlation between the doses of methadone administered and the concentrations of methadone in sweat was observed.
Schizophrenia Research | 2008
Fabien Trémeau; Melissa Brady; Erica Saccente; Alexis Moreno; Henry Epstein; Leslie Citrome; Dolores Malaspina; Daniel C. Javitt
BACKGROUND Loss aversion in decision-making refers to a higher sensitivity to losses than to gains. Loss aversion is conceived as an affective interference in cognitive processes such as judgment and decision-making. Loss aversion in non-risky choices has not been studied in schizophrenia. METHOD Forty-two individuals with schizophrenia and 42 non-patient control subjects, matched by gender and age, were randomized to two different scenarios (a buying scenario and a selling scenario). Subjects were asked to evaluate the price of a decorated mug. Schizophrenia subjects were re-tested four weeks later with the other scenario. RESULTS Contrary to non-patient controls, schizophrenia subjects did not show loss aversion. In the schizophrenia group, absence of loss aversion was correlated with age, duration of illness, number of months in State hospitals, and poorer performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, but not with current psychopathology and two domains of emotional experience. CONCLUSIONS Absence of loss aversion in schizophrenia represents a deficit in the processing of emotional information during decision-making. It can be interpreted as a lack of integration between the emotional and the cognitive systems, or to a more diffuse and de-differentiated impact of emotional information on decision-making. Future studies should bring more clarity to this question.
American Journal on Addictions | 2008
Fabien Trémeau; Angélina Darreye; Luc Staner; Humberto Correa; Hubert Weibel; Frédéric Khidichian; Jean-Paul Macher
We determined suicide attempt characteristics in 160 opioid-dependent subjects. Three aspects of suicide vulnerability were also examined: familial aggregation of suicidal behaviors, degree of aggression/impulsivity, and smoking. Forty-eight percent of subjects had a personal history of suicide attempt. A personal history of suicide attempt was associated with an early onset of heroin use, but not with gender differences. A family history of suicide was a progressive risk factor for suicide attempt. Subjects with a personal history of suicide attempt had a higher degree of aggression/impulsivity and smoked more cigarettes. In conclusion, opioid-dependent subjects who attempt suicide show familial aggregation and clinical expressions of suicidal liability similar to those described in other psychiatric groups.
Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2008
Fabien Trémeau; Michelle Goggin; Daniel Antonius; Pal Czobor; Vera Hill; Leslie Citrome
The commonly used rating scales for negative symptoms in schizophrenia have shown good reliability, but disagreement persists regarding both the content definition and the validity of several items. Instead, authors have recommended rating the specific behaviors that are defined as negative symptoms. To surmount these shortcomings, we developed a new rating scale for negative symptoms: the Motor-Affective-Social Scale (MASS). During a 5-minute structured interview, hand coverbal gestures, spontaneous smiles, voluntary smiling, and questions asked by the interviewer were counted and rated on 101 inpatients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Information on social behavior was obtained from nursing staff. The scale consisted of a total of eight items. The MASS showed high internal consistency (Cronbach alpha coefficient=0.81), inter-rater reliability, and test-retest reliability (intra-class correlation coefficient=0.81). Convergent validity analyses showed high correlations between MASS scores and scores on the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptom (SANS), and the negative symptoms subscale of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). The MASS showed excellent psychometric properties, practicality, and subject tolerability. Future research that includes the use of the MASS with other patient populations and that investigates the scales sensitivity during clinical trials should be performed.
European Neuropsychopharmacology | 2001
Humberto Correa; Fabrice Duval; Mokrani Marie Claude; P. Bailey; Fabien Trémeau; Than Son Diep; Marc-Antoine Crocq; Juarez Oliveira Castro; Jean-Paul Macher
The purpose of this study was to investigate differences in outcome following treatment with two different antidepressants in depressed patients according to their pretreatment hormonal response to clonidine. In all, 62 drug-free DSM-IV recurrent major depressed patients and 20 normal controls were studied. Patients were subsequently treated for 4 weeks with fluoxetine (n=28), or amitriptyline (n=34), and were then classified as responders or nonresponders according to their final Hamilton depression scale score. Compared to controls, depressed patients showed lower GH response to CLO (DeltaGH) (P<0.0002). One control (5%) and 35 depressed patients (56%) had blunted DeltaGH values. The efficacy of the two antidepressants was not significantly different: 15 patients responded to AMI (44%), seven patients responded to FLUOX (25%) (P>0.15). However, in the subgroup of patients with blunted DeltaGH levels, the rate of responders was higher for AMI (11/21) compared to FLUOX (1/14) treated patients (P<0.01). These results suggest that in depressed patients a blunted GH response to CLO could predict antidepressant response.
Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria | 2011
Maila de Castro Lourenço das Neves; Fabien Trémeau; Rodrigo Nicolato; Hélio Lauar; Marco Aurélio Romano-Silva; Humberto Correa
OBJECTIVE A large body of evidence suggests that several aspects of face processing are impaired in autism and that this impairment might be hereditary. This study was aimed at assessing facial emotion recognition in parents of children with autism and its associations with a functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter (5HTTLPR). METHOD We evaluated 40 parents of children with autism and 41 healthy controls. All participants were administered the Penn Emotion Recognition Test (ER40) and were genotyped for 5HTTLPR. RESULTS Our study showed that parents of children with autism performed worse in the facial emotion recognition test than controls. Analyses of error patterns showed that parents of children with autism over-attributed neutral to emotional faces. We found evidence that 5HTTLPR polymorphism did not influence the performance in the Penn Emotion Recognition Test, but that it may determine different error patterns. CONCLUSION Facial emotion recognition deficits are more common in first-degree relatives of autistic patients than in the general population, suggesting that facial emotion recognition is a candidate endophenotype for autism.