Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Fallon R. Goodman is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Fallon R. Goodman.


Psychological Assessment | 2016

Different Types of Well-Being? A Cross-Cultural Examination of Hedonic and Eudaimonic Well-Being

David J. Disabato; Fallon R. Goodman; Todd B. Kashdan; Jerome L. Short; Aaron Jarden

A large international sample was used to test whether hedonia (the experience of positive emotional states and satisfaction of desires) and eudaimonia (the presence of meaning and development of ones potentials) represent 1 overarching well-being construct or 2 related dimensions. A latent correlation of .96 presents negligible evidence for the discriminant validity between Dieners (1984) subjective well-being model of hedonia and Ryffs (1989) psychological well-being model of eudaimonia. When compared with known correlates of well-being (e.g., curiosity, gratitude), eudaimonia and hedonia showed very similar relationships, save goal-directed will and ways (i.e., hope), a meaning orientation to happiness, and grit. Identical analyses in subsamples of 7 geographical world regions revealed similar results around the globe. A single overarching construct more accurately reflects hedonia and eudaimonia when measured as self-reported subjective and psychological well-being. Nevertheless, measures of eudaimonia may contain aspects of meaningful goal-directedness unique from hedonia. (PsycINFO Database Record


Emotion | 2014

A Contextual Approach to Experiential Avoidance and Social Anxiety: Evidence From an Experimental Interaction and Daily Interactions of People With Social Anxiety Disorder

Todd B. Kashdan; Fallon R. Goodman; Kyla A. Machell; Evan M. Kleiman; Samuel S. Monfort; Joseph Ciarrochi; John B. Nezlek

Experiential avoidance (EA), the tendency to avoid internal, unwanted thoughts and feelings, is hypothesized to be a risk factor for social anxiety. Existing studies of experiential avoidance rely on trait measures with minimal contextual consideration. In two studies, we examined the association between experiential avoidance and anxiety within real-world social interactions. In the first study, we examined the effect of experiential avoidance on social anxiety in everyday life. For 2 weeks, 37 participants with Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) and 38 healthy controls provided reports of experiential avoidance and social anxiety symptoms during face-to-face social interactions. Results showed that momentary experiential avoidance was positively related to anxiety symptoms during social interactions and this effect was stronger among people with SAD. People low in EA showed greater sensitivity to the level of situational threat than high EA people. In the second study, we facilitated an initial encounter between strangers. Unlike Study 1, we experimentally created a social situation where there was either an opportunity for intimacy (self-disclosure conversation) or no such opportunity (small-talk conversation). Results showed that greater experiential avoidance during the self-disclosure conversation temporally preceded increases in social anxiety for the remainder of the interaction; no such effect was found in the small-talk conversation. Our findings provide insight into the association between experiential avoidance on social anxiety in laboratory and naturalistic settings, and demonstrate that the effect of EA depends upon level of social threat and opportunity.


Cognition & Emotion | 2015

Experiential avoidance and well-being: a daily diary analysis.

Kyla A. Machell; Fallon R. Goodman; Todd B. Kashdan

Experiential avoidance (EA) is a regulatory strategy characterised by efforts to control or avoid unpleasant thoughts, feelings and bodily sensations. Most studies of EA have used trait measures without considering the effects of EA on psychological functioning in naturalistic settings. To address this gap, we used daily diary methodology to examine the influence of EA of anxiety on everyday well-being. For two weeks, 89 participants provided daily reports of EA, positive and negative affect, enjoyment of daily events and meaning in life (MIL). Daily EA predicted higher negative affect, lower positive affect, less enjoyment of daily events (exercising, eating food and listening to music) and less MIL. The effect of EA on positive affect was not accounted for by the amount of negative affect experienced. Our daily measure of EA was a stronger predictor of daily well-being than a traditional trait measure (The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire). Taken together, results offer insights into the adverse effects of EA on daily well-being and suggest that EA is a context-specific regulatory strategy that might be best captured using a state-dependent measure.


Journal of Personality | 2016

What Triggers Anger in Everyday Life? Links to the Intensity, Control, and Regulation of These Emotions, and Personality Traits

Todd B. Kashdan; Fallon R. Goodman; Travis T. Mallard; C. Nathan DeWall

Why do people experience anger? Most of our knowledge on anger-triggering events is based on the study of reactions at a single time point in a persons life. Little research has examined how people experience anger in their daily life over time. In this study, we conducted a comprehensive examination of the situational determinants of anger over the course of 3 weeks. Using daily diary methodology, people (N = 173; 2,342 anger episodes) reported their most intense daily anger and, with an open-ended format, described the trigger. Participants also answered questions on anger intensity, control, and regulatory strategies, along with baseline personality trait measures. Using an iterative coding system, five anger trigger categories emerged: other people, psychological and physical distress, intrapersonal demands, environment, and diffuse/undifferentiated/unknown. Compared with other triggers, when anger was provoked by other people or when the source was unknown, there was a stronger positive association with anger intensity and lack of control. Personality traits (i.e., anger, mindfulness, psychological need satisfaction, the Big Five) showed few links to the experience and regulation of daily anger. Although aversive events often spur anger, the correlates and consequences of anger differ depending on the source of aversion; personality traits offer minimal value in predicting anger in daily life.


The Journal of Positive Psychology | 2018

Measuring well-being: A comparison of subjective well-being and PERMA

Fallon R. Goodman; David J. Disabato; Todd B. Kashdan; Scott Barry Kauffman

Abstract We compared Seligman’s PERMA model of well-being with Diener’s model of subjective well-being (SWB) to determine if the newer PERMA captured a type of well-being unique from the older SWB. Participants were 517 adults who completed self-report measures of SWB, PERMA, and VIA character strengths. Results from four analytic techniques suggest the factor underlying PERMA is capturing the same type of well-being as SWB. Confirmatory factor analysis yielded a latent correlation of r = 0.98 between SWB and PERMA. Exploratory structural equation modeling found two highly related factors (r = 0.85) that did not map onto PERMA and SWB. SWB and PERMA factors showed similar relationships with 24 character strengths (average correlation difference = 0.02). Latent profile analyses yielded subgroups of people who merely scored high, low, or mid-range on well-being indicators. Our findings suggest that while lower-order indicators SWB and PERMA have unique features, they converge onto a single well-being factor.


Journal of Personality | 2017

Personality Strengths as Resilience: A One‐Year Multiwave Study

Fallon R. Goodman; David J. Disabato; Todd B. Kashdan; Kyla A. Machell

We examined how personality strengths prospectively predict reactions to negative life events. Participants were 797 community adults from 42 countries. At five points over the course of 1 year, participants completed a series of questionnaires measuring seven personality strengths (hope, grit, meaning in life, curiosity, gratitude, control beliefs, and use of strengths), subjective well-being, and frequency and severity of negative life events. Using hierarchical linear modeling with assessment periods nested within participants, results from lagged analyses found that only hope emerged as a resilience factor. To illustrate the importance of using appropriate lagged analyses in resilience research, we ran nonlagged analyses; these results suggest that all seven personality strengths moderated the effect of negative life events on subjective well-being, with greater strengths associated with healthier outcomes. To provide evidence that personality strengths confer resilience, a prospective examination is needed with the inclusion of events and responses to them. The use of concurrent methodologies and analyses, which is the norm in psychology, often leads to erroneous conclusions. Hope, the ability to generate routes to reach goals and the motivation to use those routes, was shown to be particularly important in promoting resilience.


Cognition & Emotion | 2015

Perceived responsiveness during an initial social interaction with a stranger predicts a positive memory bias one week later

Evan M. Kleiman; Todd B. Kashdan; Samuel S. Monfort; Kyla A. Machell; Fallon R. Goodman

Prior research has found that perceiving positive responses from others following self-disclosures enhances social bonds and plays a role in the maintenance of romantic relationships. We sought to extend this effect by exploring perceived responsiveness to good news in the context of initial social interactions with a stranger. In this study, unacquainted college students (n = 106) participated in a 45-minute semi-structured social interaction, and information on their emotions and behaviours was collected immediately after and one week later. We found that the receipt of supportive reactions to self-disclosure attempts during the social interaction was associated with immediate positivity and a more positive memory of the event (remembered enjoyment and positive emotions) one week later. This effect could not be attributed to how positively the event was experienced immediately afterwards, suggesting that perceived responsiveness during an initial social interaction facilitates a positive memory bias. These results offer new insights into how friendships might develop and be maintained.


Journal of Personality | 2018

Is grit relevant to well-being and strengths? Evidence across the globe for separating perseverance of effort and consistency of interests

David J. Disabato; Fallon R. Goodman; Todd B. Kashdan

OBJECTIVE Researchers conceptualize grit as the combination of two facets: perseverance of effort and consistency of interests toward long-term goals. We tested the reliability of grit facet scores across the globe and examined how differently each grit facet related to well-being and personality strengths. METHOD An international sample of 7,617 participants from six of the seven continents (excluding Antarctica) completed an online survey. RESULTS Confirmatory factor analyses and omega reliability coefficients indicated that the 12 items from the original Grit Scale were multidimensional and reliably measured perseverance of effort and consistency of interests. Concurrent validity analyses showed that perseverance of effort was moderately to strongly related to subjective well-being, beliefs about well-being, and personality strengths, whereas consistency of interests had weak or negative correlations with these outcomes. The stronger relations with perseverance of effort were replicated across seven regions of the world. The presence of overall grit was supported in individualistic countries, but not collectivistic countries (i.e., those in Latin America and Asia). CONCLUSIONS We discuss the multidimensionality of grit, including a conceptual understanding of overall grit and how it may differ across cultures. We suggest well-being and strengths researchers study grit facets separately due to their differential validity.


Psychological Inquiry | 2015

Lumping and Splitting in the Study of Meaning in Life: Thoughts on Surfing, Surgery, Scents, and Sermons

Todd B. Kashdan; Jonathan Rottenberg; Fallon R. Goodman; David J. Disabato; Ena Begovic

For thousands of years, philosophers have been debating what a meaningful life entails and the best way to create one. In their article, Garland, Farb, Goldin, and Fredrickson (this issue) offer a comprehensive account of one such pathway that originates with the act of mindfulness. Specifically, when people are faced with negative life events, they should deploy mindfulness techniques to receive a chain of benefits, which include a positive reappraisal of said events, that in turn increase the likelihood of positive emotions, which can then be savored and ultimately transformed into a greater sense of meaning and purpose in life (see Figure 1 in Garland et al., this issue). This pathway has been anointed the mindfulness-to-meaning theory. In this commentary, we place this theory in a wider perspective and consider several neglected issues regarding how mindfulness may relate to meaning and purpose in life. First, meaning in life is irreducible to a single pathway. We reintroduce the concept of equifinality where diverse pathways, including chance events, can be substituted to attain the same goal (e.g., Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). We illustrate and lay out several different ways that meaning in life can be obtained. Mindfulness to meaning is integrated into a wider review of how people can create meaning in life. Second, we reintroduce the concept of multifinality (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011) where the same initial conditions, in this case mindfulness and positive reappraisal, may lead to a variety of outcomes—and only one of them is a greater sense of meaning in life. We question whether science requires a new theory about each individual mindfulness outcome. In our view, it would be a greater scientific advancement to delineate the full range of outcomes afforded by a particular behavior (the benefits and the costs) and to specify the contexts that the benefits (or the costs) might be stronger or weaker. Third, we question the widespread assumption about positivity that more is always better. Instead, we offer an alternative view on the importance of situational sensitivity, inspired by a growing body of work suggesting that psychological flexibility trumps allegiance to any single behavior or strategy such as mindfulness (Aldao, 2013; Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Kashdan & Biswas-Diener, 2014; Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010). Taken together, we hope to integrate several isolated strands of study into a nuanced discussion of mindfulness and meaning.


The Journal of Positive Psychology | 2018

Integrating psychological strengths under the umbrella of personality science: Rethinking the definition, measurement, and modification of strengths

Fallon R. Goodman; David J. Disabato; Todd B. Kashdan

ABSTRACT Following the advent of modern positive psychology, there has been a surge of empirical research on strengths and a call for incorporating strengths into clinical models of psychopathology. In this review, we conceptualize strengths as a subset of personality traits and dissect the criteria used to define strengths. In hopes of improving theoretical models of strengths, we reconsider the personal choice to deploy strengths, the implications of strength use for well-being, and the costs of over-relying on particular strengths. As an illustration, we critically examine a new model of strengths with suggestions for defining, measuring, and developing interventions for strengths. These insights are offered to encourage critical examination of the conditions under which strengths best facilitate well-being.

Collaboration


Dive into the Fallon R. Goodman's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dariusz Drążkowski

Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznań

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lukasz D. Kaczmarek

Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznań

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge