Faye J. Thompson
University of Exeter
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Faye J. Thompson.
Archive | 2016
Michael A. Cant; Hazel J. Nichols; Faye J. Thompson; Emma Vitikainen; Walter D. Koenig; Janis L. Dickinson
Th e banded mongoose ( Mungos mungo) is a small (~1.5 kg) cooperative mammal (Carnivora: Herpestidae) which is distributed widely throughout sub-Saharan Africa ( Figure 18.1 ). Th e species has been studied at sites in the Serengeti (Waser et al. 1995 ), South Africa (Hiscocks and Perrin 1991 ), and Botswana (Alexander et al. 2002 , 2010 ; Laver et al. 2012 ), but most of what is known about the life history and social behavior of this species comes from a long-term study of a population living on and around Mweya peninsula in western Uganda. Jon Rood of the Smithsonian Institute initiated study of banded mongooses at Mweya in the early 1970s and provided tantalizing insights into its social and reproductive behavior. For example, Rood confi rmed earlier reports that multiple females in each group became pregnant in each breeding attempt (Rood 1975 ); discovered that males guard young off spring at the den while lactating females go off to forage (Rood 1974 ); and described striking examples of altruism, such as one case where an adult mongoose scaled a tree to rescue a groupmate from the clutches (literally) of a martial eagle (Rood 1983 ). Unfortunately, political instability in the region prevented further work until the early 1990s, when Daniela de Luca from the Institute of Zoology in London returned to continue Rood’s research (De Luca and Ginsberg 2001 ). Th e current project was started in 1995 by the senior author and Tim Clutton-Brock, and the population has been studied continuously since then. Banded mongooses: demography, life history, and social behavior
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences | 2016
Faye J. Thompson; Harry H. Marshall; Jennifer L. Sanderson; Emma Vitikainen; Hazel J. Nichols; Jason S. Gilchrist; Andrew J. Young; Sarah J. Hodge; Michael A. Cant
In many vertebrate societies, forced eviction of group members is an important determinant of population structure, but little is known about what triggers eviction. Three main explanations are: (i) the reproductive competition hypothesis, (ii) the coercion of cooperation hypothesis, and (iii) the adaptive forced dispersal hypothesis. The last hypothesis proposes that dominant individuals use eviction as an adaptive strategy to propagate copies of their alleles through a highly structured population. We tested these hypotheses as explanations for eviction in cooperatively breeding banded mongooses (Mungos mungo), using a 16-year dataset on life history, behaviour and relatedness. In this species, groups of females, or mixed-sex groups, are periodically evicted en masse. Our evidence suggests that reproductive competition is the main ultimate trigger for eviction for both sexes. We find little evidence that mass eviction is used to coerce helping, or as a mechanism to force dispersal of relatives into the population. Eviction of females changes the landscape of reproductive competition for remaining males, which may explain why males are evicted alongside females. Our results show that the consequences of resolving within-group conflict resonate through groups and populations to affect population structure, with important implications for social evolution.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2017
Faye J. Thompson; Michael A. Cant; Harry H. Marshall; Emma Vitikainen; Jennifer L. Sanderson; Hazel J. Nichols; Jason S. Gilchrist; Matthew B.V. Bell; Andrew J. Young; Sarah J. Hodge; Rufus A. Johnstone
Significance Kin selection theory predicts that animals will direct altruism toward closer genetic relatives and aggression toward more distantly related individuals. Our 18-y study of wild banded mongooses reveals that, unusually, dominant individuals target females who are more closely related to them for violent eviction from the group. This puzzling result can be explained by selection for unrelated individuals to resist eviction and for related individuals to submit more easily. In support of this idea, we show that kin are targeted for aggression only when individuals are capable of resisting. Our results suggest that, where potential victims can oppose aggression, the usual predictions of kin selection theory can be reversed. Kin selection theory predicts that, where kin discrimination is possible, animals should typically act more favorably toward closer genetic relatives and direct aggression toward less closely related individuals. Contrary to this prediction, we present data from an 18-y study of wild banded mongooses, Mungos mungo, showing that females that are more closely related to dominant individuals are specifically targeted for forcible eviction from the group, often suffering severe injury, and sometimes death, as a result. This pattern cannot be explained by inbreeding avoidance or as a response to more intense local competition among kin. Instead, we use game theory to show that such negative kin discrimination can be explained by selection for unrelated targets to invest more effort in resisting eviction. Consistent with our model, negative kin discrimination is restricted to eviction attempts of older females capable of resistance; dominants exhibit no kin discrimination when attempting to evict younger females, nor do they discriminate between more closely or less closely related young when carrying out infanticidal attacks on vulnerable infants who cannot defend themselves. We suggest that in contexts where recipients of selfish acts are capable of resistance, the usual prediction of positive kin discrimination can be reversed. Kin selection theory, as an explanation for social behavior, can benefit from much greater exploration of sequential social interactions.
Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution | 2016
Emma Vitikainen; Michael A. Cant; Jennifer L. Sanderson; Christopher Mitchell; Hazel J. Nichols; Harry H. Marshall; Faye J. Thompson; Jason S. Gilchrist; Sarah J. Hodge; Rufus A. Johnstone; Jonathan D. Blount
Oxidative damage has been proposed as a potential mechanism underlying a life history tradeoff between survival and reproduction. However, evidence that reproduction is associated with increased oxidative damage is equivocal, and some studies have found that breeding females exhibit reduced, rather than elevated, levels of oxidative damage compared to equivalent non-breeders. Recently it was hypothesised that oxidative damage could have negative impacts on developing offspring, and that mothers might down-regulate oxidative damage during reproduction to shield their offspring from such damage. We tested this hypothesis through a longitudinal study of adult survival, reproduction, and oxidative damage in wild banded mongooses (Mungos mungo) in Uganda. High levels of oxidative damage as measured by malondialdehyde (MDA) were associated with reduced survival in both sexes. Levels of protein carbonyls were not linked to survival. Mothers showed reduced levels of MDA during pregnancy, and individuals with higher MDA levels gestated fewer offspring and had lower pup survival. These results suggest that maternal oxidative damage has transgenerational costs, and are consistent with the idea that mothers may attempt to shield their offspring from particularly harmful types of oxidative damage during pregnancy. Further advance in understanding of life history variation would benefit from theory and tests of the potential transgenerational impacts of the mechanisms underlying life history trade-offs.
Behavioral Ecology | 2016
Harry H. Marshall; Jennifer L. Sanderson; Francis Mwanghuya; Robert Businge; Solomon Kyabulima; Michelle C. Hares; Emma Inzani; Gladys Kalema-Zikusoka; Kenneth Mwesige; Faye J. Thompson; Emma Vitikainen; Michael A. Cant
Lay Summary Male banded mongooses babysit more when rainfall is variable. Banded mongooses live in cooperative family groups and males in particular help raise pups that are not necessarily their own. It has been suggested that ecological conditions affect cooperation, and our study confirms that the variability of conditions is important: Females face higher mortality during years with more variable rainfall, and males may be better off helping their relatives when there are fewer opportunities for mating. Twitter: @HarryHMarshall
Animal Behaviour | 2017
Faye J. Thompson; Harry H. Marshall; Emma Vitikainen; Michael A. Cant
Conflict between groups is a notable feature of many animal societies. Recent theoretical models suggest that violent intergroup conflict can shape patterns of within-group cooperation. However, despite its prevalence in social species, the adaptive significance of violent intergroup conflict has been little explored outside of humans and chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. A barrier to current understanding of the role of intergroup conflict in the evolution of social behaviour is a lack of information on the causes and consequences of aggression between groups. Here, we examined the causes and fitness consequences of intergroup conflict in the banded mongoose, Mungos mungo, using a 16-year data set of observed intergroup interactions, life history and behaviour. Banded mongooses are cooperative breeders that live in highly territorial groups and engage in frequent, aggressive and violent intergroup interactions. We found that intensified population-wide competition for food and mates increased the probability of intergroup interactions, and that increased intergroup conflict was associated with periods in which groups were growing in size. Intergroup conflict had fitness costs in terms of reduced litter and adult survival but no cost to pregnant females: in fact, females were less likely to abort following an intergroup interaction than when there had been no recent intergroup conflict. Our results suggest that intergroup conflict has measurable costs to both individuals and groups in the long and short term, and that levels of conflict among groups could be high enough to affect patterns of within-group cooperative behaviour. Establishing the consequences of intergroup conflict in cooperative species can shed light on patterns of conflict and cooperation within groups and, in turn, facilitate our understanding of social evolution.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences | 2017
Emma Vitikainen; Harry H. Marshall; Faye J. Thompson; Jenni L. Sanderson; Matthew B.V. Bell; Jason S. Gilchrist; Sarah J. Hodge; Hazel J. Nichols; Michael A. Cant
Kin selection theory predicts that animals should direct costly care where inclusive fitness gains are highest. Individuals may achieve this by directing care at closer relatives, yet evidence for such discrimination in vertebrates is equivocal. We investigated patterns of cooperative care in banded mongooses, where communal litters are raised by adult ‘escorts’ who form exclusive caring relationships with individual pups. We found no evidence that escorts and pups assort by parentage or relatedness. However, the time males spent escorting increased with increasing relatedness to the other group members, and to the pup they had paired with. Thus, we found no effect of relatedness in partner choice, but (in males) increasing helping effort with relatedness once partner choices had been made. Unexpectedly, the results showed clear assortment by sex, with female carers being more likely to tend to female pups, and male carers to male pups. This sex-specific assortment in helping behaviour has potential lifelong impacts on individual development and may impact the future size and composition of natal groups and dispersing cohorts. Where relatedness between helpers and recipients is already high, individuals may be better off choosing partners using other predictors of the costs and benefits of cooperation, without the need for possibly costly within-group kin discrimination.
Scientific Reports | 2016
Emma Inzani; Harry H. Marshall; Jennifer L. Sanderson; Hazel J. Nichols; Faye J. Thompson; Gladys Kalema-Zikusoka; Sarah J. Hodge; Michael A. Cant; Emma Vitikainen
Female intrasexual competition is intense in cooperatively breeding species where offspring compete locally for resources and helpers. In mammals, females have been proposed to adjust prenatal investment according to the intensity of competition in the postnatal environment (a form of ‘predictive adaptive response’; PAR). We carried out a test of this hypothesis using ultrasound scanning of wild female banded mongooses in Uganda. In this species multiple females give birth together to a communal litter, and all females breed regularly from one year old. Total prenatal investment (size times the number of fetuses) increased with the number of potential female breeders in the group. This relationship was driven by fetus size rather than number. The response to competition was particularly strong in low weight females and when ecological conditions were poor. Increased prenatal investment did not trade off against maternal survival. In fact we found the opposite relationship: females with greater levels of prenatal investment had elevated postnatal maternal survival. Our results support the hypothesis that mammalian prenatal development is responsive to the intensity of postnatal competition. Understanding whether these responses are adaptive requires information on the long-term consequences of prenatal investment for offspring fitness.
Ecology and Evolution | 2017
Harry H. Marshall; Emma Vitikainen; Francis Mwanguhya; Robert Businge; Solomon Kyabulima; Michelle C. Hares; Emma Inzani; Gladys Kalema-Zikusoka; Kenneth Mwesige; Hazel J. Nichols; Jennifer L. Sanderson; Faye J. Thompson; Michael A. Cant
Abstract Early‐life ecological conditions have major effects on survival and reproduction. Numerous studies in wild systems show fitness benefits of good quality early‐life ecological conditions (“silver‐spoon” effects). Recently, however, some studies have reported that poor‐quality early‐life ecological conditions are associated with later‐life fitness advantages and that the effect of early‐life conditions can be sex‐specific. Furthermore, few studies have investigated the effect of the variability of early‐life ecological conditions on later‐life fitness. Here, we test how the mean and variability of early‐life ecological conditions affect the longevity and reproduction of males and females using 14 years of data on wild banded mongooses (Mungos mungo). Males that experienced highly variable ecological conditions during development lived longer and had greater lifetime fitness, while those that experienced poor early‐life conditions lived longer but at a cost of reduced fertility. In females, there were no such effects. Our study suggests that exposure to more variable environments in early life can result in lifetime fitness benefits, whereas differences in the mean early‐life conditions experienced mediate a life‐history trade‐off between survival and reproduction. It also demonstrates how early‐life ecological conditions can produce different selection pressures on males and females.
Ecology Letters | 2018
Catherine Sheppard; Richard Inger; Robbie A. McDonald; Sam Barker; Andrew L. Jackson; Faye J. Thompson; Emma Vitikainen; Michael A. Cant; Harry H. Marshall
Abstract Individual foraging specialisation has important ecological implications, but its causes in group‐living species are unclear. One of the major consequences of group living is increased intragroup competition for resources. Foraging theory predicts that with increased competition, individuals should add new prey items to their diet, widening their foraging niche (‘optimal foraging hypothesis’). However, classic competition theory suggests the opposite: that increased competition leads to niche partitioning and greater individual foraging specialisation (‘niche partitioning hypothesis’). We tested these opposing predictions in wild, group‐living banded mongooses (Mungos mungo), using stable isotope analysis of banded mongoose whiskers to quantify individual and group foraging niche. Individual foraging niche size declined with increasing group size, despite all groups having a similar overall niche size. Our findings support the prediction that competition promotes niche partitioning within social groups and suggest that individual foraging specialisation may play an important role in the formation of stable social groupings.