Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Flip Phillips is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Flip Phillips.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1995

The perception of surface orientation from multiple sources of optical information

J. Farley Norman; James T. Todd; Flip Phillips

An orientation matching task was used to evaluate observers’ sensitivity to local surface orientation at designated probe points on randomly shaped 3-D objects that were optically defined by texture, lambertian shading, or specular highlights. These surfaces could be stationary or in motion, and they could be viewed either monocularly or stereoscopically, in all possible combinations. It was found that the deformations of shading and/or highlights (either over time or between the two eyes’ views) produced levels of performance similar to those obtained for the optical deformations of textured surfaces. These findings suggest that the human visual system utilizes a much richer array of optical information to support its perception of shape than is typically appreciated.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 1998

The perception of surface curvature from optical motion

Victor Perotti; James T. Todd; Joseph S. Lappin; Flip Phillips

Observers viewed the optical flow field of a rotating quadric surface patch and were required to match its perceived structure by adjusting the shape of a stereoscopically presented surface. In Experiment 1, the flow fields included rigid object rotations and constant flow fields with patterns of image acceleration that had no possible rigid interpretation. In performing their matches, observers had independent control of two parameters that determined the surface shape. One of these, called the shape characteristic, is defined as the ratio of the two principle curvatures and is independent of object size. The other, called curvedness, is defined as the sum of the squared principle curvatures and depends on the size of the object. Adjustments of shape characteristic were almost perfectly accurate for both motion conditions. Adjustments of curvedness, on the other hand, were systematically overestimated and were not highly correlated with the simulated curvedness of the depicted surface patch. In Experiment 2, the same flow fields were masked with a global pattern of curl, divergence, or shear, which disrupted the first-order spatial derivatives of the image velocity field, while leaving the second-order spatial derivatives invariant. The addition of these masks had only negligible effects on observers’ performance. These findings suggest that observers’ judgments of three-dimensional surface shape from motion are primarily determined by the second-order spatial derivatives of the instantaneous field of image displacements.


Perception | 2001

Information Concentration along the Boundary Contours of Naturally Shaped Solid Objects

J. Farley Norman; Flip Phillips; Heather E Ross

In this study of the informativeness of boundary contours for the perception of natural object shape, observers viewed shadows/silhouettes cast by natural solid objects and were required to adjust the positions of a set of 10 points so that the resulting dotted shape resembled the shape of the original silhouette as closely as possible. For each object, the observers were then asked to indicate the corresponding positions of the 10 points on the original boundary contour. The results showed that there was a close correspondence between the chosen positions of the points and the locations along the boundary contour that were local curvature maxima (convexities or concavities). This finding differs from that of Kennedy and Domander (1985 Perception 14 367–370), and shows that, at least for natural objects, the original hypothesis of Attneave (1954 Psychological Review 61 183–193) is valid—local curvature maxima are indeed important for the perception of shape.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1996

Surface range and attitude probing in stereoscopically presented dynamic scenes

Jan J. Koenderink; Astrid M. L. Kappers; James T. Todd; J. Farley Norman; Flip Phillips

The authors report on different methods to probe the structure of visually perceived surfaces in 3 dimensions. The surfaces are specified by patterns of shading with Lambertian and specular components, which deform over time and over stereoscopic views. Five observers performed 2 probe tasks, 1 involving the adjustment of a punctate probe so as to be on the apparent surface and the other involving the adjustment of a small gauge figure that indicates surface attitude. The authors found that these rather different methods yielded essentially identical depth maps up to linear transformations and that the observers all deviate slightly from veridicality in basically identical ways. The nature of this deviation appears to be correlated with the rough topography of the specularities.


Perception | 1998

The Perception of Scale-Dependent and Scale-Independent Surface Structure from Binocular Disparity, Texture, and Shading

James S. Tittle; J. Farley Norman; Victor Perotti; Flip Phillips

The integration of binocular disparity, shading, and texture was measured for two different aspects of three-dimensional structure: (1) shape index, which is a measure of scale-independent structure, and (2) curvedness, which is a measure of scale-dependent structure. Binocular disparity was found to contribute significantly more to judged shape index than it does to judged curvedness, and shading and texture were both found to contribute more to judged curvedness than to judged shape index. These results demonstrate that different cues do not contribute equally to different aspects of perceived surface structure. This finding suggests that, for the case of linear integration, multiple cues to three-dimensional structure do not combine on the basis of a single type of representation shared by all the ‘shape-from-X’ processes in the visual system.


Attention Perception & Psychophysics | 2003

Perceptual representation of visible surfaces

Flip Phillips; James T. Todd; Jan J. Koenderink; Astrid M. L. Kappers

Two experiments are reported in which we examined the ability of observers to identify landmarks on surfaces from different vantage points. In Experiment 1, observers were asked to mark the local maxima and minima of surface depth, whereas in Experiment 2, they were asked to mark the ridges and valleys on a surface. In both experiments, the marked locations were consistent across different observers and remained reliably stable over different viewing directions. These findings indicate that randomly generated smooth surface patches contain perceptually salient landmarks that have a high degree of viewpoint invariance. Implications of these findings are considered for the recognition of smooth surface patches and for the depiction of such surfaces in line drawings.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1996

PERCEPTION OF LOCAL THREE-DIMENSIONAL SHAPE

Flip Phillips; James T. Todd

The authors present a series of 4 experiments designed to test the ability to perceive local shape information. Observers were presented with various smoothly varying 3-dimensional surfaces where they reported shape index and sign of Gaussian curvature at several probe locations. Results show that observers are poor at making judgments based on these local measures, especially when the region surrounding the local point is restricted or manipulated to make it noncoherent. Shape index judgments required at least 2 degrees of context surrounding the probe location, and performance on sign of Gaussian curvature judgments deteriorated as the contextual information was restricted as well.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 1997

Perceptual localization of surface position

Flip Phillips; James T. Todd; Jan J. Koenderink; Astrid M. L. Kappers

In 2 experiments, observers were required to identify corresponding points on an object viewed from multiple orientations. On each trial, a surface was presented initially with a single target location marked by a small dot. Following a brief blank interval, the same surface was presented again at a different orientation. The observer was required to position an adjustable probe dot in this 2nd display to match the location of the target in the 1st view. Under optimal conditions, the variance in their settings over multiple trials was just a few minutes of arc, though these errors varied significantly with the structural complexity of the depicted surface.


Experimental Brain Research | 2012

Solid shape discrimination from vision and haptics: natural objects (Capsicum annuum) and Gibson’s “feelies”

J. Farley Norman; Flip Phillips; Jessica Holmin; Hideko F. Norman; Amanda M. Beers; Alexandria Boswell; Jacob R. Cheeseman; Angela G. Stethen; Cecilia Ronning

A set of three experiments evaluated 96 participants’ ability to visually and haptically discriminate solid object shape. In the past, some researchers have found haptic shape discrimination to be substantially inferior to visual shape discrimination, while other researchers have found haptics and vision to be essentially equivalent. A primary goal of the present study was to understand these discrepant past findings and to determine the true capabilities of the haptic system. All experiments used the same task (same vs. different shape discrimination) and stimulus objects (James Gibson’s “feelies” and a set of naturally shaped objects—bell peppers). However, the methodology varied across experiments. Experiment 1 used random 3-dimensional (3-D) orientations of the stimulus objects, and the conditions were full-cue (active manipulation of objects and rotation of the visual objects in depth). Experiment 2 restricted the 3-D orientations of the stimulus objects and limited the haptic and visual information available to the participants. Experiment 3 compared restricted and full-cue conditions using random 3-D orientations. We replicated both previous findings in the current study. When we restricted visual and haptic information (and placed the stimulus objects in the same orientation on every trial), the participants’ visual performance was superior to that obtained for haptics (replicating the earlier findings of Davidson et al. in Percept Psychophys 15(3):539–543, 1974). When the circumstances resembled those of ordinary life (e.g., participants able to actively manipulate objects and see them from a variety of perspectives), we found no significant difference between visual and haptic solid shape discrimination.


Acta Psychologica | 2010

Does monocular visual space contain planes

Jan J. Koenderink; Liliana Albertazzi; Andrea J. van Doorn; Raymond van Ee; Wim A. van de Grind; Astrid M. L. Kappers; Joseph S. Lappin; J. Farley Norman; Augustinus H. J. Oomes; Susan P. te Pas; Flip Phillips; Sylvia C. Pont; Whitman Richards; James T. Todd; Frans A. J. Verstraten; Sjoerd de Vries

The issue of the existence of planes-understood as the carriers of a nexus of straight lines-in the monocular visual space of a stationary human observer has never been addressed. The most recent empirical data apply to binocular visual space and date from the 1960s (Foley, 1964). This appears to be both the first and the last time this basic issue was addressed empirically. Yet the question is of considerable conceptual interest. Here we report on a direct empirical test of the existence of planes in monocular visual space for a group of sixteen experienced observers. For the majority of these observers monocular visual space lacks a projective structure, albeit in qualitatively different ways. This greatly reduces the set of viable geometrical models. For example, it rules out all the classical homogeneous spaces (the Cayley-Klein geometries) such as the familiar Luneburg model. The qualitatively different behavior of experienced observers implies that the generic population might well be inhomogeneous with respect to the structure of visual space.

Collaboration


Dive into the Flip Phillips's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. Farley Norman

Western Kentucky University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Eric Egan

Ohio State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brett R. Fajen

Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hideko F. Norman

Western Kentucky University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jan J. Koenderink

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge