Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where G.A. van Kleef is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by G.A. van Kleef.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2006

Supplication and appeasement in conflict and negotiation: The interpersonal effects of disappointment, worry, guilt, and regret

G.A. van Kleef; C.K.W. de Dreu; Antony Stephen Reid Manstead

This study examined the social effects of emotions related to supplication and appeasement in conflict and negotiation. In a computer-simulated negotiation, participants in Experiment 1 were confronted with a disappointed or worried opponent (supplication), with a guilty or regretful opponent (appeasement), or with a nonemotional opponent (control). Compared with controls, participants conceded more when the other experienced supplication emotions and conceded less when the other experienced appeasement emotions (especially guilt). Experiment 2 replicated the effects of disappointment and guilt and showed that they are moderated by the perceivers dispositional trust: Negotiators high in trust conceded more to a disappointed counterpart than to a happy one, but those with low trust were unaffected. In Experiment 3, trust was manipulated through information about the others personality (cooperative vs. competitive), and a similar moderation was obtained.


Advances in Experimental Social Psychology | 2008

A reciprocal influence model of social power: Emerging principles and lines of inquiry

Dacher Keltner; G.A. van Kleef; Serena Chen; Michael W. Kraus

Abstract In the present chapter, we advance a reciprocal influence model of social power. Our model is rooted in evolutionist analyses of primate hierarchies, and notions that the capacity for subordinates to form alliances imposes important demands upon those in power, and that power heuristically reduces the likelihood of conflicts within groups. Guided by these assumptions, we posit a set of propositions regarding the reciprocal nature of power, and review recent supporting data. With respect to the acquisition of social power, we show that power is afforded to those individuals and strategic behaviors related to advancing the interests of the group. With respect to constraints upon power, we detail how group‐based representations (a fellow group members reputation), communication (gossip), and self‐assessments (an individuals modest sense of power) constrain the actions of those in power according to how they advance group interests. Finally, with respect to the notion that power acts as a social interaction heuristic, we examine how social power is readily and accurately perceived by group members and gives priority to the emotions, goals, and actions of high‐power individuals in shaping interdependent action. We conclude with a discussion of recent studies of the subjective sense of power and class‐based ideologies.


Journal of Applied Psychology | 2011

Hot or cold: is communicating anger or threats more effective in negotiation?

Marwan Sinaceur; G.A. van Kleef; Neale; H. Adam; Christophe Haag

Is communicating anger or threats more effective in eliciting concessions in negotiation? Recent research has emphasized the effectiveness of anger communication, an emotional strategy. In this article, we argue that anger communication conveys an implied threat, and we document that issuing threats is a more effective negotiation strategy than communicating anger. In 3 computer-mediated negotiation experiments, participants received either angry or threatening messages from a simulated counterpart. Experiment 1 showed that perceptions of threat mediated the effect of anger (vs. a control) on concessions. Experiment 2 showed that (a) threat communication elicited greater concessions than anger communication and (b) poise (being confident and in control of ones own feelings and decisions) ascribed to the counterpart mediated the positive effect of threat compared to anger on concessions. Experiment 3 replicated this positive effect of threat over anger when recipients had an attractive alternative to a negotiated agreement. These findings qualify previous research on anger communication in negotiation. Implications for the understanding of emotion and negotiation are discussed.


Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 2008

What Other's Disappointment May Do to Selfish People: Emotion and Social Value Orientation in a Negotiation Context:

G.A. van Kleef; P.A.M. van Lange

The authors examined whether individual differences in social value orientation moderate responses to other’s expressions of disappointment in negotiation. The literature suggested competing hypotheses: First, prosocials are more responsive to other’s disappointment because they have a greater concern for other; second, proselfs are more responsive because they see other’s disappointment as a threat to their own outcomes. Results of a computer-mediated negotiation in which a simulated opponent expressed disappointment, no emotion, or anger supported the second prediction: Proselfs conceded more to a disappointed opponent than to a neutral or angry one, whereas prosocials were unaffected by the other’s emotion. This effect was mediated by participants’ motivation to satisfy the other’s needs, which disappointment triggered more strongly in proselfs than in prosocials. Implications for theorizing on emotion, social value orientation, and negotiation are discussed.


Social Neuroscience | 2013

Behavioral and neural reactions to emotions of others in the distribution of resources

Gert-Jan Lelieveld; E. van Dijk; Berna Güroğlu; I. van Beest; G.A. van Kleef; Serge A.R.B. Rombouts; Eveline A. Crone

This study investigated the neural mechanisms involved in the interpersonal effects of emotions—i.e., how people are influenced by other peoples emotions. Participants were allocators in a version of the dictator game and made a choice between two offers after receiving written emotional expressions of the recipients. The results showed that participants more often made a self-serving offer when dealing with an angry recipient than when dealing with a happy or disappointed recipient. Compared to disappointment, expressions of anger increased activation in regions associated with self-referential thinking (anterior medial prefrontal cortex, aMPFC) and (emotional) conflict (anterior cingulate cortex). We found increased activation in temporoparietal junction for receiving happy reactions in comparison with receiving angry or disappointed reactions. This study thus emphasizes that distinct emotions have distinct effects on people in terms of behavior and underlying neurological mechanisms.


Social Psychological and Personality Science | 2015

Power Gets You High: The Powerful Are More Inspired by Themselves Than by Others

G.A. van Kleef; Christopher Oveis; Astrid C. Homan; I. van der Löwe; Dacher Keltner

Inspiration is a source of admirable creation—but where do people get it from? We propose that power allows individuals to draw inspiration from the self. Four studies involving different social settings and operationalizations support this idea. Study 1 revealed that greater power is associated with more self-derived inspiration and less other-derived inspiration. In Study 2, participants with a higher sense of power were more inspired by their own than by their partners’ stories in face-to-face conversations, whereas lower power participants were not. In Study 3, higher power people spontaneously generated more inspiring stories involving themselves than did lower power people. Finally, participants in Study 4 felt more inspired after writing about their own experiences than after writing about someone else’s, especially after having been primed with high rather than low power. These findings suggest that powerful people prioritize themselves over others in social interaction because this is emotionally rewarding for them.


Psychological Inquiry | 2017

The social effects of emotions are functionally equivalent across expressive modalities

G.A. van Kleef

Ever since the publication of Darwin’s The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals in 1872, questions about the nature and purpose of emotional expressions have represented some of the most intriguing and enduring conundrums in the social and affective sciences. How do facial displays relate to emotional experience? What constitutes an emotional expression? What do emotional expressions signal? How do our emotional expressions influence others? Here I address the latter problem, focusing in particular on the question of how the social effects of emotions, that is, the effects of one person’s emotional expressions on others, compare across different expressive modalities, including facial displays, vocal cues, bodily postures, language, and symbols such as emoticons. Moving beyond the classic questions of what emotions are, how they arise, and what they represent, contemporary theorizing and research increasingly emphasize the socialcommunicative functions and effects of emotional expressions (e.g., Fischer & Manstead, 2016; Keltner & Haidt, 1999; Parkinson, 1996; Planalp, 1999; Van Kleef, Cheshin, Fischer, & Schneider, 2016). The latest contribution to this literature is the “pragmatic” approach advocated by Andrea Scarantino (this issue), which aims to illuminate the socialcommunicative properties of emotional expressions by drawing an analogy with formal language. This focus reflects the important insight that scientific understanding of the nature of emotional expressions requires thorough consideration of their social consequences. In this commentary I compare the view of emotional expressions as communicative acts that is central to the theory of affective pragmatics (TAP; Scarantino, this issue) with the focus on the social consequences of emotional expressions that is advanced in Emotions as Social Information (EASI) theory (Van Kleef, 2009, 2016), another recent theoretical framework that emphasizes the social-communicative functions of emotions. In doing so, I focus mostly on one particular aspect in which the two perspectives seem to differ, namely, how social communication by means of nonverbal emotional expressions compares to communication through emotional language. A Pragmatic Approach to the Social-Communicative Functions of EmotionsDisclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology | 2018

The art of influence: When and why deviant artists gain impact

Eftychia Stamkou; G.A. van Kleef; Astrid C. Homan

Some artists rise to fame, while others sink into oblivion. What determines whether artists make an impact? Considering deviance in its sociohistorical context, we propose that artists whose work deviates from their own previous style (intrapersonal deviance) and other artists’ styles (interpersonal deviance) gain greater impact than nondeviant artists, as long as deviance is directed toward a progressive style. A preliminary study showed that in western cultures nonrealistic styles are considered more progressive than realistic styles (Study 1). Five more studies provide evidence for the effects of the two types of artistic deviance on several aspects of impact (i.e., perceived influence of the artist, valuation of the artwork, and visual attention to the artwork). First, individuals considered artists who deviated from their previous style more impactful than artists who consistently followed a single style (Study 2), effects that were stronger when artists transitioned from a retrogressive style to a progressive one (Study 3). Second, artists who deviated from their contemporaries’ style were considered more impactful than artists who followed the predominant style, effects that were stronger when artists strayed from a predominant retrogressive style by using progressive means of expression (Studies 4 and 5). When the historical context prevented observers from inferring the progressiveness of the deviant artists’ expressive means, artistic deviance enhanced perceived impact regardless of the means by which the artists deviated (Study 6). Supporting our theoretical model, the effects of intrapersonal and interpersonal deviance on impact were mediated by perceived will-power (Studies 3, 5, and 6).


Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology | 2018

Seeing Mixed Emotions: The Specificity of Emotion Perception From Static and Dynamic Facial Expressions Across Cultures

X. Fang; Disa Sauter; G.A. van Kleef

Although perceivers often agree about the primary emotion that is conveyed by a particular expression, observers may concurrently perceive several additional emotions from a given facial expression. In the present research, we compared the perception of two types of nonintended emotions in Chinese and Dutch observers viewing facial expressions: emotions which were morphologically similar to the intended emotion and emotions which were morphologically dissimilar to the intended emotion. Findings were consistent across two studies and showed that (a) morphologically similar emotions were endorsed to a greater extent than dissimilar emotions and (b) Chinese observers endorsed nonintended emotions more than did Dutch observers. Furthermore, the difference between Chinese and Dutch observers was more pronounced for the endorsement of morphologically similar emotions than of dissimilar emotions. We also obtained consistent evidence that Dutch observers endorsed nonintended emotions that were congruent with the preceding expressions to a greater degree. These findings suggest that culture and morphological similarity both influence the extent to which perceivers see several emotions in a facial expression.


Archive | 2015

Power Gets You High: The Powerful Are More Inspired by Themselves Than by Others - eScholarship

G.A. van Kleef; Christopher Oveis; Astrid C. Homan; I. van der Löwe; Dacher Keltner

Inspiration is a source of admirable creation—but where do people get it from? We propose that power allows individuals to draw inspiration from the self. Four studies involving different social settings and operationalizations support this idea. Study 1 revealed that greater power is associated with more self-derived inspiration and less other-derived inspiration. In Study 2, participants with a higher sense of power were more inspired by their own than by their partners’ stories in face-to-face conversations, whereas lower power participants were not. In Study 3, higher power people spontaneously generated more inspiring stories involving themselves than did lower power people. Finally, participants in Study 4 felt more inspired after writing about their own experiences than after writing about someone else’s, especially after having been primed with high rather than low power. These findings suggest that powerful people prioritize themselves over others in social interaction because this is emotionally rewarding for them.

Collaboration


Dive into the G.A. van Kleef's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dacher Keltner

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge