Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Gerine M. A. Lodder is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Gerine M. A. Lodder.


Perspectives on Psychological Science | 2015

Loneliness Across the Life Span

Pamela Qualter; Janne Vanhalst; Rebecca Harris; Eeske van Roekel; Gerine M. A. Lodder; Munirah Bangee; Marlies Maes; Maaike Verhagen

Most people have experienced loneliness and have been able to overcome it to reconnect with other people. In the current review, we provide a life-span perspective on one component of the evolutionary theory of loneliness—a component we refer to as the reaffiliation motive (RAM). The RAM represents the motivation to reconnect with others that is triggered by perceived social isolation. Loneliness is often a transient experience because the RAM leads to reconnection, but sometimes this motivation can fail, leading to prolonged loneliness. We review evidence of how aspects of the RAM change across development and how these aspects can fail for different reasons across the life span. We conclude with a discussion of age-appropriate interventions that may help to alleviate prolonged loneliness.


Nature Human Behaviour | 2018

Justify your alpha

Daniël Lakens; Federico G. Adolfi; Casper J. Albers; Farid Anvari; Matthew A. J. Apps; Shlomo Argamon; Thom Baguley; Raymond Becker; Stephen D. Benning; Daniel E. Bradford; Erin M. Buchanan; Aaron R. Caldwell; Ben Van Calster; Rickard Carlsson; Sau Chin Chen; Bryan Chung; Lincoln John Colling; Gary S. Collins; Zander Crook; Emily S. Cross; Sameera Daniels; Henrik Danielsson; Lisa M. DeBruine; Daniel J. Dunleavy; Brian D. Earp; Michele I. Feist; Jason D. Ferrell; James G. Field; Nicholas W. Fox; Amanda Friesen

In response to recommendations to redefine statistical significance to P ≤ 0.005, we propose that researchers should transparently report and justify all choices they make when designing a study, including the alpha level.


British Journal of Psychology | 2016

Loneliness and the social monitoring system: Emotion recognition and eye gaze in a real-life conversation

Gerine M. A. Lodder; Ron H. J. Scholte; Luc Goossens; Rutger C. M. E. Engels; Maaike Verhagen

Based on the belongingness regulation theory (Gardner et al., 2005, Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull., 31, 1549), this study focuses on the relationship between loneliness and social monitoring. Specifically, we examined whether loneliness relates to performance on three emotion recognition tasks and whether lonely individuals show increased gazing towards their conversation partners faces in a real-life conversation. Study 1 examined 170 college students (Mage = 19.26; SD = 1.21) who completed an emotion recognition task with dynamic stimuli (morph task) and a micro(-emotion) expression recognition task. Study 2 examined 130 college students (Mage = 19.33; SD = 2.00) who completed the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test and who had a conversation with an unfamiliar peer while their gaze direction was videotaped. In both studies, loneliness was measured using the UCLA Loneliness Scale version 3 (Russell, 1996, J. Pers. Assess., 66, 20). The results showed that loneliness was unrelated to emotion recognition on all emotion recognition tasks, but that it was related to increased gaze towards their conversation partners faces. Implications for the belongingness regulation system of lonely individuals are discussed.


Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology | 2017

Loneliness in Early Adolescence: Friendship Quantity, Friendship Quality, and Dyadic Processes.

Gerine M. A. Lodder; Ron H. J. Scholte; Luc Goossens; Maaike Verhagen

Friendship quantity and quality are related to adolescent loneliness, but the exact link between these constructs is not well understood. The present study aimed to examine whether adolescents’ perception of friendship quantity and quality, and the perceptions of their peers, were related to loneliness. We examined the relation between loneliness and the number of unilateral and reciprocal friendships and compared the views of best friendship quality. Overall, 1,172 Dutch adolescents (49.1% male, M age = 12.81, SD = .43) nominated their friends and rated their friendship quality. Friendship quantity was measured using sociometrics to distinguish reciprocated and unilateral (i.e., one-sided) friendships. The analyses indicated that loneliness was related to fewer reciprocal and unilateral-received friendships (i.e., the adolescent received a friendship nomination but did not reciprocate that nomination) and a lower quality of best friendship. Actor–partner interdependence analyses revealed that adolescents’ loneliness was related to a less positive evaluation of their friendship, as reported by adolescents themselves (i.e., a significant actor effect) but not by their friends (i.e., nonsignificant partner effect). These findings (a) indicate that loneliness is negatively related to the number of friends adolescents have, as perceived by themselves and their peers and (b) suggest that, once a friendship is established, lonely adolescents may interpret the friendship quality less positively compared to their friends. Implications of these findings for our current understanding of adolescent loneliness are discussed, and suggestions for future research are outlined.


PLOS ONE | 2015

Loneliness and hypervigilance to social cues in females: an eye-tracking study.

Gerine M. A. Lodder; Ron H. J. Scholte; Ivar A. H. Clemens; Rutger C. M. E. Engels; Luc Goossens; Maaike Verhagen

The goal of the present study was to examine whether lonely individuals differ from nonlonely individuals in their overt visual attention to social cues. Previous studies showed that loneliness was related to biased post-attentive processing of social cues (e.g., negative interpretation bias), but research on whether lonely and nonlonely individuals also show differences in an earlier information processing stage (gazing behavior) is very limited. A sample of 25 lonely and 25 nonlonely students took part in an eye-tracking study consisting of four tasks. We measured gazing (duration, number of fixations and first fixation) at the eyes, nose and mouth region of faces expressing emotions (Task 1), at emotion quadrants (anger, fear, happiness and neutral expression) (Task 2), at quadrants with positive and negative social and nonsocial images (Task 3), and at the facial area of actors in video clips with positive and negative content (Task 4). In general, participants tended to gaze most often and longest at areas that conveyed most social information, such as the eye region of the face (T1), and social images (T3). Participants gazed most often and longest at happy faces (T2) in still images, and more often and longer at the facial area in negative than in positive video clips (T4). No differences occurred between lonely and nonlonely participants in their gazing times and frequencies, nor at first fixations at social cues in the four different tasks. Based on this study, we found no evidence that overt visual attention to social cues differs between lonely and nonlonely individuals. This implies that biases in social information processing of lonely individuals may be limited to other phases of social information processing. Alternatively, biased overt attention to social cues may only occur under specific conditions, for specific stimuli or for specific lonely individuals.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2016

Lower Sensitivity to Happy and Angry Facial Emotions in Young Adults with Psychiatric Problems

Charlotte Vrijen; Catharina A. Hartman; Gerine M. A. Lodder; Maaike Verhagen; Peter de Jonge; Albertine J. Oldehinkel

Many psychiatric problem domains have been associated with emotion-specific biases or general deficiencies in facial emotion identification. However, both within and between psychiatric problem domains, large variability exists in the types of emotion identification problems that were reported. Moreover, since the domain-specificity of the findings was often not addressed, it remains unclear whether patterns found for specific problem domains can be better explained by co-occurrence of other psychiatric problems or by more generic characteristics of psychopathology, for example, problem severity. In this study, we aimed to investigate associations between emotion identification biases and five psychiatric problem domains, and to determine the domain-specificity of these biases. Data were collected as part of the ‘No Fun No Glory’ study and involved 2,577 young adults. The study participants completed a dynamic facial emotion identification task involving happy, sad, angry, and fearful faces, and filled in the Adult Self-Report Questionnaire, of which we used the scales depressive problems, anxiety problems, avoidance problems, Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) problems and antisocial problems. Our results suggest that participants with antisocial problems were significantly less sensitive to happy facial emotions, participants with ADHD problems were less sensitive to angry emotions, and participants with avoidance problems were less sensitive to both angry and happy emotions. These effects could not be fully explained by co-occurring psychiatric problems. Whereas this seems to indicate domain-specificity, inspection of the overall pattern of effect sizes regardless of statistical significance reveals generic patterns as well, in that for all psychiatric problem domains the effect sizes for happy and angry emotions were larger than the effect sizes for sad and fearful emotions. As happy and angry emotions are strongly associated with approach and avoidance mechanisms in social interaction, these mechanisms may hold the key to understanding the associations between facial emotion identification and a wide range of psychiatric problems.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2015

The Possible Antecedents and Consequences of Matching of Food Intake: Examining the Role of Trait Self-Esteem and Interpersonal Closeness

Elizabeth Hirata; Gerine M. A. Lodder; Ulrich Kühnen; Sonia Lippke; Roel C.J. Hermans

Although there is evidence that people tend to match their intake to that of others, less is known about the motives underlying this effect. The current study, therefore, examined the relationship between self-esteem, a specific factor that has been related to the likelihood of social matching. Further, we examined the effects of food matching on interpersonal closeness among eating companions. The sample included 89 female dyads. All dyads had free access to palatable snack food during a 15 min interaction. For each dyad the matching score was calculated, as well as both individuals trait self-esteem scores and interpersonal closeness with their eating partner. The overall degree of matching within dyads was high, replicating the findings of previous research. No relationship, however, was found between trait self-esteem and the degree of matching. Furthermore, there was no effect of matching on perceived interpersonal closeness with or liking of the other person. These results suggest that self-esteem might not be a robust predictor of matching and that matching of food intake may not result in increased perceived interpersonal closeness or liking among eating partners.


Journal of Youth and Adolescence | 2017

Parent-Adolescent Discrepancies Regarding Adolescents' Peer-Related Loneliness : Associations with Adolescent Adjustment

Annette Spithoven; Janne Vanhalst; Gerine M. A. Lodder; Patricia Bijttebier; Luc Goossens

Because loneliness is a subjective experience, it is often examined using self-reports. Yet, researchers have started to use other-reports to examine loneliness. As previous research suggests that discrepancies between self- and other views might have important implications for adolescents’ mental health, the current study examines discrepancies in multi-informant reports on adolescents’ loneliness in relation with prosocial behavior, aggression, and adolescents’ parent-related loneliness. The sample consisted of 374 mother-adolescent dyads and 318 father-adolescent dyads (41.80% male, Mage = 15.67 years, SD = 1.25). Results indicated that informants used different reference points to assess adolescents’ peer-related loneliness, but were otherwise comparable. Moreover, informant discrepancies were associated with greater adolescents’ reported parent-related loneliness. The current study did not provide evidence that discrepancies were related to prosocial or aggressive behavior. The current study adds to the notion that other-reports on loneliness show substantial convergence with self-reports. In addition, this study indicates that the discrepancy between other- and self-reports on loneliness holds valuable information for adolescent socio-emotional adjustment.


Journal of Research on Adolescence | 2013

Selection, Deselection, and Socialization Processes of Happiness in Adolescent Friendship Networks

Nicole van Workum; Ron H. J. Scholte; Antonius H. N. Cillessen; Gerine M. A. Lodder; Matteo Giletta


Journal of Youth and Adolescence | 2016

Bully victimization: selection and influence within adolescent friendship networks and cliques

Gerine M. A. Lodder; Ron H. J. Scholte; Antonius H. N. Cillessen; Matteo Giletta

Collaboration


Dive into the Gerine M. A. Lodder's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maaike Verhagen

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Luc Goossens

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ron H. J. Scholte

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Annette Spithoven

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Patricia Bijttebier

Catholic University of Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Janne Vanhalst

Catholic University of Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Margot Bastin

Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Albertine J. Oldehinkel

University Medical Center Groningen

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge