Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Ginette Dionne is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Ginette Dionne.


Developmental Psychology | 2003

Physical Aggression and Expressive Vocabulary in 19-Month-Old Twins

Ginette Dionne; Richard E. Tremblay; Michel Boivin; David P. Laplante; Daniel Pérusse

In the prevention of physical aggression, possible etiological links with language development are rarely taken into account. Indeed, little is known about when language and aggressive behavior become linked during development and which mechanisms are responsible for this association. This study investigated the association between physical aggression and language in late infancy with a genetic design that involved 562 19-month-old twins. A modest but significant correlation (r = -.20) was found between physical aggression and expressive vocabulary. Substantial heritability was found for physical aggression. Quantitative genetic modeling suggests that the correlation between expressive vocabulary and physical aggression cannot be explained by shared etiologies. However, phenotype-to-phenotype models indicate that the covariation can be entirely accounted for by a significant phenotypic path from expressive vocabulary to physical aggression. The implications of these results for early prevention of chronic physical aggression are discussed.


Child Development | 2003

Genetic Evidence for Bidirectional Effects of Early Lexical and Grammatical Development

Ginette Dionne; Philip S. Dale; Michel Boivin; Robert Plomin

This article addresses the autonomy hypothesis of vocabulary and grammar and bootstrapping mechanisms in early language development. Two birth cohorts of 1,505 and 1,049 same-sex twin pairs from the UK were assessed at 2 and 3 years on grammar and vocabulary, using adapted versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. Vocabulary and grammar correlate strongly at both 2 and 3 years in both cohorts. Multivariate genetic modeling reveals a consistently high genetic correlation between vocabulary and grammar at 2 and 3 years. This finding suggests the same genetic influences operate for both vocabulary and grammar, a finding incompatible with traditional autonomy hypothesis, at least in early acquisition. Crosslagged longitudinal genetic models indicate both lexical and syntactical bootstrapping operate from 2 to 3 years.


Developmental Science | 2000

Sex differences in early verbal and non‐verbal cognitive development

M J Galsworthy; Ginette Dionne; Philip S. Dale; Robert Plomin

The present study of over 3000 2-year-old twin pairs used a sex-limitation model to examine genetic and environmental origins of sex differences in verbal and non-verbal cognitive ability. Girls scored significantly higher on both measures (p<0.0001), although gender only accounted for approximately 3% of the variance in verbal ability and 1% of the variance in non-verbal cognitive ability. For the verbal measure boys showed greater heritability than girls. Also the twin-pair correlation is significantly lower for opposite-sex twins than for non-identical same-sex twins. This indicates that individual differences in verbal ability include some sex-specific factors. Non-verbal cognitive ability did not differ in aetiology for boys and girls. We conclude that genetic and environmental influences differ for girls and boys for early verbal but not non-verbal development.


Journal of Child Language | 2000

Lexical and Grammatical Development: A Behavioural Genetic Perspective.

Philip S. Dale; Ginette Dionne; Thalia C. Eley; Robert Plomin

The relation of lexical and grammatical knowledge is at the core of many controversies in linguistics and psycholinguistics. Recent empirical findings that the two are highly correlated in early language development have further energized the theoretical debate. Behavioural genetics provides an illuminating new tool to explore this question, by addressing the question of whether the empirical correlation simply reflects the fact that environments which facilitate one aspect of language growth also facilitate the other, or whether the same underlying acquisition mechanisms, influenced by the same genes, are responsible for the correlation. We explored this issue in a study of 2898 pairs of two-year-old twins born in England and Wales. Language development was assessed by their parents using an adapted version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory which assesses vocabulary and grammar. Moderate heritabilities were found for both. As in previous studies, measures of vocabulary and sentence complexity were substantially correlated (r = 0.66). Behaviour-genetic modelling of the relation of vocabulary and grammar produced an estimated value of 0.61 for the genetic correlation, a measure of the overlap of the genetic effects that contribute to the two aspects of language development. In contrast, a measure of nonverbal cognitive development, the PARCA, was only weakly correlated at both the phenotypic level and at the level of genetic correlations with the language measures. Thus, although the distinction between verbal and nonverbal skills has a genetic basis underlying the phenotypic dissociation, there is little evidence either genetically or phenotypically for a dissociation between vocabulary and grammar within language.


Archives of General Psychiatry | 2008

Variations in Heritability of Cortisol Reactivity to Stress as a Function of Early Familial Adversity Among 19-Month-Old Twins

Isabelle Ouellet-Morin; Michel Boivin; Ginette Dionne; Sonia J. Lupien; Louise Arsenault; Ronald G. Barr; Daniel Pérusse; Richard E. Tremblay

CONTEXT Cortisol reactivity is a marker of vulnerability for a variety of stress-related diseases that likely arise from the contributions of both genetic and environmental sources of influence. However, little is known about gene-environment interplay in early cortisol reactivity. OBJECTIVES To examine the genetic and environmental contributions to early cortisol reactivity in a population-based sample of 19-month-old twins and to determine whether these contributions vary as a function of early familial adversity. DESIGN A variant of the twin method, with genetic and environment contributions to cortisol reactivity estimated as a function of familial adversity. Familial adversity was defined as the presence of 7 risk factors during perinatal and postnatal development (eg, at 6 and 19 months of age): maternal smoking during pregnancy, low birth weight, low family income, low maternal educational level, single parenthood, young motherhood, and maternal hostile or reactive behaviors. Twins exposed to 4 or more risk factors at either time were considered as having been exposed to high (vs low) familial adversity (23.4% of the sample). SETTING Centre de Recherche Fernand-Seguin at the Hôpital Louis-Hyppolite Lafontaine, Montréal, Quebec. Patients Participants were families of twins from the Québec Newborn Twin Study recruited between April 1, 1995, and December 31, 1998, in the greater Montréal area. A total of 346 twins, 130 monozygotic and 216 dizygotic, were included in the study. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Salivary cortisol samples were collected before and after the participating twins had been exposed to unfamiliar situations; change in cortisol over time was used as a measure of cortisol reactivity. RESULTS Distinct patterns of genetic and environmental contributions to cortisol reactivity were evidenced as a function of familial adversity, suggesting a possible gene-environment interplay. In low-familial adversity settings that characterized most families, both genetic and unique but not shared environmental factors accounted for individual differences in cortisol reactivity, with shared genes explaining the similarity observed within twin pairs. By contrast, in conditions of high familial adversity, both shared and unique environmental factors, but not genetic factors, accounted for the variance in cortisol reactivity. CONCLUSION This pattern of differing genetic and environmental contributions according to familial adversity suggests that, early in life, high familial adversity may have a programming developmental effect on cortisol reactivity.


Child Development | 2009

Early Child Language Mediates the Relation Between Home Environment and School Readiness

Nadine Forget-Dubois; Ginette Dionne; Jean-Pascal Lemelin; Daniel Pérusse; Richard E. Tremblay; Michel Boivin

Home environment quality is a well-known predictor of school readiness (SR), although the underlying processes are little known. This study tested two hypotheses: (a) child language mediates the association between home characteristics (socioeconomic status and exposure to reading) and SR, and (b) genetic factors partly explain the association between language and SR. Data were collected between 6 and 63 months in a large sample of twins. Results showed that home characteristics had direct effects on SR and indirect effects through child language. No genetic correlation was found between language and SR. These results suggest that home characteristics affect SR in part through their effect on early language skills, and show that this process is mainly environmental rather than genetic in nature.


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry | 2010

Closing the gap in academic readiness and achievement: the role of early childcare

Marie-Claude Geoffroy; Sylvana M. Côté; Charles Édouard Giguère; Ginette Dionne; Philip David Zelazo; Richard E. Tremblay; Michel Boivin; Jean R. Séguin

BACKGROUND Socially disadvantaged children with academic difficulties at school entry are at increased risk for poor health and psychosocial outcomes. Our objective is to test the possibility that participation in childcare--at the population level--could attenuate the gap in academic readiness and achievement between children with and without a social disadvantage (indexed by low levels of maternal education). METHODS A cohort of infants born in the Canadian province of Quebec in 1997/1998 was selected through birth registries and followed annually until 7 years of age (n = 1,863). Children receiving formal childcare (i.e., center-based or non-relative out-of-home) were distinguished from those receiving informal childcare (i.e., relative or nanny). Measures from 4 standardized tests that assessed cognitive school readiness (Lollipop Test for School Readiness), receptive vocabulary (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Revised), mathematics (Number Knowledge Test), and reading performance (Kaufman Assessment Battery for children) were administered at 6 and 7 years. RESULTS Children of mothers with low levels of education showed a consistent pattern of lower scores on academic readiness and achievement tests at 6 and 7 years than those of highly educated mothers, unless they received formal childcare. Specifically, among children of mothers with low levels of education, those who received formal childcare obtained higher school readiness (d = 0.87), receptive vocabulary (d = 0.36), reading(d = 0.48) and math achievement scores (d = 0.38; although not significant at 5%) in comparison with those who were cared for by their parents. Childcare participation was not associated with cognitive outcomes among children of mothers with higher levels of education. CONCLUSIONS Public investments in early childcare are increasing in many countries with the intention of reducing cognitive inequalities between disadvantaged and advantaged children. Our findings provide further evidence suggesting that formal childcare could represent a preventative means of attenuating effects of disadvantage on childrens early academic trajectory.


Developmental Psychology | 2006

Examining genetic and environmental effects on reactive versus proactive aggression

Mara Brendgen; Frank Vitaro; Michel Boivin; Ginette Dionne; Daniel Pérusse

This study compared the contribution of genes and environment to teacher-rated reactive and proactive aggression in 6-year-old twin pairs (172 pairs: 55 monozygotic girls, 48 monozygotic boys, 33 dizygotic girls, 36 dizygotic boys). Genetic effects accounted for 39% of the variance of reactive aggression and for 41% of the variance of proactive aggression. The remainder of the variance was explained by unique environmental effects. Genetic as well as unique environmental effects were significantly correlated across reactive and proactive aggression (genetic correlation = .87, environmental correlation = .34), but this overlap was largely due to a common underlying form of aggression (i.e., teacher-rated physical aggression). Once common etiological factors due to physical aggression were accounted for, reactive and proactive aggression shared no other genes and only a few environmental influences, although additional specific genetic and environmental effects were observed for both reactive and proactive aggression. These specific effects indicate that both reactive and proactive aggression may be influenced mostly by socialization experiences that are specific to each type of aggression and only to a very small degree by specific genes.


Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology | 2013

Cohort Profile: The Maternal-Infant Research on Environmental Chemicals Research Platform

Tye E. Arbuckle; William D. Fraser; Mandy Fisher; Karelyn Davis; Chun Lei Liang; Nicole Lupien; Stéphanie Bastien; M.P. Vélez; Peter von Dadelszen; Denise G. Hemmings; Jingwei Wang; Michael Helewa; Shayne Taback; Mathew Sermer; Warren G. Foster; Greg Ross; Paul Fredette; Graeme N. Smith; Mark Walker; Roberta Shear; Linda Dodds; Adrienne S. Ettinger; Jean-Philippe Weber; Monique D'Amour; Melissa Legrand; Premkumari Kumarathasan; Renaud Vincent; Zhong-Cheng Luo; Robert W. Platt; Grant Mitchell

BACKGROUND The Maternal-Infant Research on Environmental Chemicals (MIREC) Study was established to obtain Canadian biomonitoring data for pregnant women and their infants, and to examine potential adverse health effects of prenatal exposure to priority environmental chemicals on pregnancy and infant health. METHODS Women were recruited during the first trimester from 10 sites across Canada and were followed through delivery. Questionnaires were administered during pregnancy and post-delivery to collect information on demographics, occupation, life style, medical history, environmental exposures and diet. Information on the pregnancy and the infant was abstracted from medical charts. Maternal blood, urine, hair and breast milk, as well as cord blood and infant meconium, were collected and analysed for an extensive list of environmental biomarkers and nutrients. Additional biospecimens were stored in the studys Biobank. The MIREC Research Platform encompasses the main cohort study, the Biobank and follow-up studies. RESULTS Of the 8716 women approached at early prenatal clinics, 5108 were eligible and 2001 agreed to participate (39%). MIREC participants tended to smoke less (5.9% vs. 10.5%), be older (mean 32.2 vs. 29.4 years) and have a higher education (62.3% vs. 35.1% with a university degree) than women giving birth in Canada. CONCLUSIONS The MIREC Study, while smaller in number of participants than several of the international cohort studies, has one of the most comprehensive datasets on prenatal exposure to multiple environmental chemicals. The biomonitoring data and biological specimen bank will make this research platform a significant resource for examining potential adverse health effects of prenatal exposure to environmental chemicals.


Pediatrics | 2008

Gestational Diabetes Hinders Language Development in Offspring

Ginette Dionne; Michel Boivin; Jean R. Séguin; Daniel Pérusse; Richard E. Tremblay

BACKGROUND. Previous studies have suggested that language is affected in infants of diabetic mothers, yet there have been no systematic investigations to address this question. OBJECTIVE. Our goal was to compare infants of diabetic mothers and controls on language outcomes from ages 18 months to 7 years. METHODS. This was a case-control longitudinal design with 2 birth cohorts: 1835 singletons from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (born October 1997 to July 1998) and 998 twins from the Quebec Newborn Twin Study (born November 1995 to July 1998). Cases were 221 infants of diabetic mothers (105 singletons and 116 twins), and controls were 2612 children (1730 singletons and 882 twins) for whom at least 1 language measure from ages 18 months to 7 years was available. Exclusion criteria were gestation of <32 weeks. The outcome measures were McArthur Communicative Development Inventory expressive and receptive vocabulary and grammar at 18 months and 30 months, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test receptive vocabulary at 48 months and expressive and receptive vocabulary at 60 months, and Early Development Instrument teacher-assessed communication at 72 months and 84 months (kindergarten and first grade). RESULTS. Analyses of variance (controlling for gender, socioeconomic status, and perinatal factors) revealed effects of gestational diabetes on expressive language at 18, 30, and 72/84 months. Infants of diabetic mothers scored 0.27 to 0.41 SD lower than controls and were 2.2 times more at risk of a language impairment. Genes and maternal education both moderated the effect of gestational diabetes on expressive language during this period. CONCLUSION. Gestational diabetes hinders expressive language in offspring into middle childhood. Genes are strongly associated with the risk of delays in infants of diabetic mothers, and offspring of educated mothers are less affected.

Collaboration


Dive into the Ginette Dionne's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Frank Vitaro

Carnegie Mellon University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Mara Brendgen

Université du Québec à Montréal

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alain Girard

Université du Québec à Montréal

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge