Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Guy Beugnon is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Guy Beugnon.


The Journal of Experimental Biology | 2012

Ants might use different view-matching strategies on and off the route

Antoine Wystrach; Guy Beugnon; Ken Cheng

SUMMARY Individual foraging ants are known to rely on views of their surroundings for route learning and for pinpointing goals. Different strategies have been proposed to explain how ants might process visual information for navigation, but little is known about the actual development and nature of the view-based strategies used by ants in complex natural environments. Here, we constrained the knowledge of Melophorus bagoti ants to either the nest vicinity or a curved route (length 10 m) and analysed their initial direction when released at both novel and familiar locations. In parallel, we used 360 deg pictures of the scene as a basis for modelling different navigational strategies. We propose here a new hypothesis based on skyline height comparison to explain how ants home from novel locations. Interestingly, this strategy succeeded well at novel locations but failed on familiar terrain. By contrast, the use of a visual compass strategy failed at novel locations but could explain the results on familiar routes. We suggest that ants might switch between skyline height comparison and a visual compass strategy, depending on whether they are on familiar terrain or not. How ants could switch between strategies and how their memories develop are discussed in turn.


Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 2011

Views, landmarks, and routes : how do desert ants negotiate an obstacle course?

Antoine Wystrach; Sebastian Schwarz; Patrick Schultheiss; Guy Beugnon; Ken Cheng

The Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti often follows stereotypical routes through a cluttered landscape containing both distant panoramic views and obstacles (plants) to navigate around. We created an artificial obstacle course for the ants between a feeder and their nest. Landmarks comprised natural objects in the landscape such as logs, branches, and tussocks. Many ants travelled stereotypical routes home through the obstacle course in training, threading repeatedly the same gaps in the landmarks. Manipulations altering the relations between the landmarks and the surrounding panorama, however, affected the routes in two major ways. Both interchanging the positions of landmarks (transpositions) and displacing the entire landmark set along with the starting position of the ants (translations) (1) reduced the stereotypicality of the route, and (2) increased turns and meanders during travel. The ants might have used the entire panorama in view-based travel, or the distal panorama might prime the identification and use of landmarks en route. Despite the large data set, both options (not mutually exclusive) remain viable.


Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology | 1997

Graded recruitment and hunting strategies linked to prey weight and size in the ponerine ant Ectatomma ruidum

Bertrand Schatz; Jean-Paul Lachaud; Guy Beugnon

Abstract According to the weight and size of their prey, Ectatomma ruidum workers can employ different recruitment systems (solitary hunting, cooperative hunting and group hunting with recruitment) when mastering and retrieving prey items from short distances from the nest. Prey size determined the backwards entry typically adopted by this species, while prey weight determined the predatory strategy selected. After a common initial sequence (search for prey, detection, localization), predatory sequences varied in terms of the type of approach, the site of seizure, the reaction after stinging and the type of transport. Nevertheless, irrespective of prey weight and size, seizure was preferentially oriented towards the head and prey were always stung. Short-range recruitment and mass recruitment without trail laying were elicited by a large range of heavy prey (> 2.5 times the weight of an individual worker). According to the mortality risk associated with each prey, hunters exhibited a “prudent” stinging posture associated with an increase in the duration of the subsequent phase of waiting for prey immobilization. The overall time of capture was positively correlated with the weight of the prey. When collective hunting strategies were involved, E. ruidum colonies matched the number of recruited hunters to the size and weight of the prey. Compared to solitary hunting strategies, for short food–nest distances, this graded recruitment appeared to enhance the energetic benefits derived by this species from the use of recruitment systems: the higher the number of workers involved in the recruitment process, the greater the energetic benefits obtained. The exhibition or absence of trail laying behavior in the recruitment responses displayed by E. ruidum workers is discussed in relation to their involvement in scavenging or predatory behavior.


Journal of Ethology | 1993

Efficiency in the exploitation of patchy environments by the ponerine antPaltothyreus tarsatus: an ecological consequence of the flexibility of prey capture behavior

Alain Dejean; Jean-Paul Lachaud; Guy Beugnon

Paltothyreus tarsatus workers show an adaptive predatory strategy compatible with central place theory which predicts that single-prey loading is an extension of the optimal diet choice while multiple-prey loading behavior would correspond to the optimal use of patches. The insight learning involved in the quick modifications of predatory strategy enablesP. tarsatus to hunt all available prey in a great diversity of sizes and species. Nevertheless, this generalist predator strongly preferred termites and very large prey such as giant diplopods and crickets to other choices within its diet. In the hunting of these favorite prey, the recruitment of nestmates enhanced the efficiency of total predation, though the release of a chemical trail appeared to depend on the hunger-satiety balance of the colony. In addition to the hunger, the miscapture of prey also triggered the release of chemical trails. The strategy for capturing grouped termites was characterized by the loading of multiple prey at a single time, by a concentrated search in a restricted area and by an optional recruitment of nestmates. These behavioral characteristics of ponerine ants probably account for the flexibility of their predatory strategy for hunting aggregated small prey.


Insectes Sociaux | 1992

Adaptative properties of the chemical trail system of the African weaver antOecophylla longinoda Latreille (Hymenoptera, Formicidae, Formicinae)

Guy Beugnon; Alain Dejean

SummaryTrail communication of the weaver antOecophylla longinoda is highly adapted to the African rain forests, insofar natural selection in the tropics might favour chemical trails which are durable in nature. A dry chemical trail can last over nine weeks, and over ten months when reinforced with fecal marking. The trail pheromones are resistant to rain, whether they are fresh or three months old, and whether they are reinforced or not with anal deposits.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 1998

The learning of a sequence of visual patterns by the ant Cataglyphis cursor

Stéphane Chameron; Bertrand Schatz; Isabelle Pastergue-Ruiz; Guy Beugnon; Thomas S. Collett

We used a maze to explore the ability of Cataglyphis cursor to store multiple visual patterns presented in a fixed sequence. Ants were trained individually to negotiate a linear maze that consisted of four boxes connected by tunnels and through which an ant travelled from a sucrose feeder back to its nest. Each box had one entrance and two possible exits. One exit led to a blocked tunnel and the other to an open tunnel leading to the entrance of the next box. The open and closed exits in each box were labelled by different solid, black shapes that were specific to each box. Ants learnt to negotiate the maze using the shapes for guidance rather than a fixed motor strategy. Trained ants could not only discriminate positive from negative shapes, but had also learnt which positive shape belonged to which box. For example, when the positive shape appropriate to box 1 (1+) was pitted against that appropriate to box 3 (3+), ants preferred 1+ to 3+ in box 1, but chose 3+ over 1+ in box 3. We conclude that ants can identify individual positive shapes and expect to encounter them in the correct order independently of extra-maze cues.


Journal of Insect Behavior | 2005

Use of Long-Term Stored Vector Information in the Neotropical Ant Gigantiops destructor

Guy Beugnon; Jean-Paul Lachaud; Philippe Chagné

We investigated how the formicine ant Gigantiops destructor can use vector information to navigate within the cluttered environment of the rain forest. Displaced foragers use skylight information to move in the theoretical feeder-to-nest direction, whether they are prevented from updating their path-integrator during foraging or captured at the departure from their nest, i.e. with a current accumulator state very close to zero. Only ants that have collected food are able to download a long-term stored reference vector pointing in the nest direction, irrespective of the current accumulator state of their path-integrator stored in a working memory and independent of familiar landmarks. Depending on the release sites, ants that became lost at a maximum distance of 50 cm could still hit and recognize their familiar route, or they engaged in a systematic search for it centered on the release sites. In contrast to Cataglyphis desert ants, Gigantiops ants do not rely primarily on the current accumulator state of their egocentric path integrator. Such a long-term vector-based navigation primed by food capture is well adapted for a tropical ant foraging during periods spanning several hours. This could prevent the numerous cumulative errors in the evaluation of the angles steered that might result from a continuously running path-integrator operating during complex foraging patterns performed at ground or arboreal levels and during passive displacement in response to heavy rain.


Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 2006

Ant navigation en route to the goal: signature routes facilitate way-finding of Gigantiops destructor

D. Macquart; L. Garnier; M. Combe; Guy Beugnon

We investigated in laboratory conditions how foragers of the tropical ant Gigantiopsdestructor develop individually distinctive landmark routes. Way-finding along a familiar route involved the recognition of at least two locations, nest and feeding site, and the representation of spatial relations between these places. Familiar visual landmarks were important both at the beginning and at the end of the foraging journey. A motor routine guided the ants at the start of their foraging path towards the first landmarks, which they learnt to pass consistently on the same side, before taking the next direction. At the last stage of the route, landmark recognition allowed them to pinpoint their preferred feeding site without using distant cues or odometric information. By contrast, ants en route to the goal were not systematically guided by a stereotyped sequence of snapshots recalled at each corresponding stage of the route. Each ant slalomed in an idiosyncratic distinctive way around different midway landmarks from a foraging excursion to the next, which induced a variability of the path shapes in their intermediate parts. By reducing the number of landmark recognition-triggered responses, this economical visuomotor strategy may be helpful in the Amazonian forest where many prominent landmarks are alike.


Advances in The Study of Behavior | 1987

Ontogenetic development of behavior: The cricket visual world

Raymond Campan; Guy Beugnon; Michel Lambin

Publisher Summary This chapter lies within such a theoretical framework— that is, constructivist, systemic, and self-organizing with respect, necessarily, to a limited behavioral target: the ontogenic construction of the visual world of the cricket. At first, the chapter deals with the developmental changes of some psychophysiological conditions controlling the selection of visual information; these factors are only parts of the various interacting elements within the individual subsystem. Then it is studied, from the same ontogenetic viewpoint, some complex, interactive, and highly variable ecological conditions within the environmental subsystem. Of course such an approach cannot be exhaustive. However, it is considered that the eventual involvement of new factors of a psychophysiological or environmental nature, which remains possible at any time, would affect only slightly the general processes of behavioral ontogeny. In the third part, the history of a number of behavioral emergents within such a developing visual world is reported. Finally, an attempt is made to derive the consequences of model by drawing several possible general rules governing the behavioral changes in the course of individual development. In this chapter, the deductive principles are presented first by stating the theoretical framework of the research; thereafter the research is presented.


Journal of Insect Behavior | 1989

Homing in the field cricket,Gryllus campestris

Guy Beugnon; Raymond Campan

Homing ability of the cricket Gryllus campestriswas experimentally studied in the field after passive and active displacements. Whatever the weather conditions and the nature of the displacement, crickets (18/18) home directly when they are located between 5 and 15 cm from their burrow by performing real-time orientation. When moved by the experimenter between 15 and 55 cm away from home, some crickets (26/43) can retrieve their burrow by systematic searching only after winding inward trips but they get lost (0/18) beyond 55 cm. After self-generated movements between 5 and 85 cm away from home, crickets (33/35) return to their burrow under blue sky, but they cannot home as well under overcast sky (22/34 from 5 to 65 cm and 0/5 beyond). The volatile memory orientation system allowing path integration is based on the polarized skylight of the sun.

Collaboration


Dive into the Guy Beugnon's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bertrand Schatz

University of Montpellier

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jérôme Orivel

Centre national de la recherche scientifique

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

E. Le Bourg

Paul Sabatier University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Raymond Campan

Paul Sabatier University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David Macquart

Paul Sabatier University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge