Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Hanna Damasio is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Hanna Damasio.


Neurobiology of Aging | 2005

Normal neuroanatomical variation due to age: the major lobes and a parcellation of the temporal region.

John S. Allen; Joel Bruss; C. Kice Brown; Hanna Damasio

We used high-resolution MRI to investigate gray and white matter aging in the major lobes of the cerebrum (frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital) and the major sectors of the temporal lobe (temporal pole, superior temporal gyrus, infero-temporal region, parahippocampal gyrus, amygdala, hippocampus). Subjects included 87 adults between the ages of 22 and 88 years. Regions of interest were hand-traced on contiguous 1.5mm coronal slices. For the cerebrum in general, gray matter decreased linearly with age, resulting in a decline of about 9.1-9.8% between the ages of 30 and 70 years, and a decline of 11.3-12.3% by the age of 80. In contrast, white matter volume increased until the mid-50s, after which it declined at an accelerated rate. At 70 years, white matter volume was only 5.6-6.4% less than at 30 years, but by age 80, a cubic regression model predicted that the decrease would be 21.6-25.0%. Multivariate analyses indicate that the frontal gray matter was most strongly associated with age, while occipital gray and white matter were least associated. Reduction in volume in the hippocampus was best modeled by a cubic regression model rather than a linear model. No sex differences in aging were found for any regions of interest.


NeuroImage | 2003

Sexual dimorphism and asymmetries in the gray–white composition of the human cerebrum

John S. Allen; Hanna Damasio; Thomas J. Grabowski; Joel Bruss; Wei Zhang

Using high resolution MRI scans and automated tissue segmentation, gray and white matter (GM, WM) volumes of the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes, cingulate gyrus, and insula were calculated. Subjects included 23 male and 23 female healthy, right-handed subjects. For all structures, male volumes were greater than female, but the gray/white (G/W) ratio was consistently higher across structures in women than men. Sexual dimorphism was greater for WM than GM: most of the G/W ratio sex differences can be attributed to variation in WM volume. The corpus callosum, although larger in men, is less sexually dimorphic than the WM as a whole. Several regions demonstrate pair-wise asymmetries in G/W ratio and WM volume. Both the cingulate gyrus and insula exhibit strong asymmetries. The left cingulate gyrus is significantly larger than the right, and the G/W ratio of the left insula is significantly greater than that of the right. Although statistically significant sex differences and asymmetries are present at this level of analysis, we argue that researchers should be wary of ascribing cognitive functional significance to these patterns at this time. This is not to say, however, that these patterns are not important for understanding the natural history of the human brain, and its evolution and development.


Cognitive Neuropsychology | 2003

Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions.

Daniel Tranel; David Kemmerer; Ralph Adolphs; Hanna Damasio; Antonio R. Damasio

The neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions are not well understood. To begin to address this knowledge gap, we tested the hypothesis that the retrieval of conceptual knowledge for actions depends on neural systems located in higher-order association cortices of left premotor/prefrontal, parietal, and posterior middle temporal regions. The investigation used the lesion method and involved 90 subjects with damage to various regions of the left or right hemisphere. The experimental tasks measured retrieval of knowledge for actions, in a nonverbal format: Subjects evaluated attributes of pictured actions, and compared and matched pictures of actions. In support of our hypothesis, we found that the regions of highest lesion overlap in subjects with impaired retrieval of conceptual knowledge for actions were in the left premotor/prefrontal sector, the left parietal region, and in the white matter underneath the left posterior middle temporal region. These sites are partially distinct from those identified previously as being important for the retrieval of words for actions. We propose that a key function of the sites is to operate as two-way intermediaries between perception and concept retrieval, to promote the retrieval of the multidimensional aspects of knowledge that are necessary and sufficient for the mental representation of a concept of a given action.


Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2004

Cortical Regions for Judgments of Emotions and Personality Traits from Point-light Walkers

Andrea S. Heberlein; Ralph Adolphs; Daniel Tranel; Hanna Damasio

Humans are able to use nonverbal behavior to make fast, reliable judgments of both emotional states and personality traits. Whereas a sizeable body of research has identified neural structures critical for emotion recognition, the neural substrates of personality trait attribution have not been explored in detail. In the present study, we investigated the neural systems involved in emotion and personality trait judgments. We used a type of visual stimulus that is known to convey both emotion and personality information, namely, point-light walkers. We compared the emotion and personality trait judgments made by subjects with brain damage to those made by neurologically normal subjects and then conducted a lesion overlap analysis to identify neural regions critical for these two tasks. Impairments on the two tasks dissociated: Some subjects were impaired at emotion recognition, but judged personality normally; other subjects were impaired on the personality task, but normal at emotion recognition. Moreover, these dissociations in performance were associated with damage to specific neural regions: Right somatosensory cortices were a primary focus of lesion overlap in subjects impaired on the emotion task, whereas left frontal opercular cortices were a primary focus of lesion overlap in subjects impaired on the personality task. These findings suggest that attributions of emotional states and personality traits are accomplished by partially dissociable neural systems.


Brain Research | 1996

A chronic microelectrode investigation of the tonotopic organization of human auditory cortex

Matthew A. Howard; Igor O. Volkov; Paul J. Abbas; Hanna Damasio; Michael C. Ollendieck; Mark A. Granner

We investigated the functional organization of human auditory cortex using a new chronic microelectrode technique. Tonotopic mapping data was obtained at the single unit level for the first time in humans. All sound-driven units were noted to have frequency-dependent response patterns. The majority of units (73%) demonstrated sharply tuned excitatory best-frequency responses. Twenty seven percent of units showed wide receptive fields, representing excitatory responses to almost the entire range of frequencies presented. A tonotopic pattern was observed with best frequencies systematically increasing as more medial-caudal recording sites were sampled.


Cortex | 1987

A Left Hemisphere Contribution to Visuospatial Processing

Ziyah Mehta; Freda Newcombe; Hanna Damasio

Men with chronic, focal, unilateral missile injures of the brain--twenty-five with left hemisphere lesions (LH group) and twenty with right hemisphere lesions (RH groups)--and twenty-two control subjects were given two visuoperceptual and two visuospatial tests. The LH group was significantly impaired in relation to the control group on both the spatial tasks. A different pattern of dissociable perceptual and spatial deficits was found in the experimental groups: better preserved perceptual than spatial performance was observed more frequently in the LH group whereas the converse--relatively better spatial than perceptual performance--was more evident in the RH group. Double dissociations in performance on the two spatial tasks were found predominantly in the LH group. These findings suggest an important left hemisphere contribution to visuospatial processing and the possibility of a more focal representation of spatial abilities in the left hemisphere than in the right.


NeuroImage | 2005

The neural correlates of spatial language in English and American Sign Language: a PET study with hearing bilinguals.

Karen Emmorey; Thomas J. Grabowski; Stephen McCullough; Laura L. Boles Ponto; Richard D. Hichwa; Hanna Damasio

Rather than specifying spatial relations with a closed-class set of prepositions, American Sign Language (ASL) encodes spatial relations using space itself via classifier constructions. In these constructions, handshape morphemes specify object type, and the position of the hands in signing space schematically represents the spatial relation between objects. A [15O]water PET study was conducted to investigate the neural regions engaged during the production of English prepositions and ASL locative classifier constructions in hearing subjects with deaf parents (ASL-English bilinguals). Ten subjects viewed line drawings depicting a spatial relation between two objects and were asked to produce either an ASL locative classifier construction or an English preposition that described the spatial relation. The comparison task was to name the figure object (colored red) in either ASL or in English. Describing spatial relations in either ASL or English engaged parietal cortex bilaterally. However, an interaction analysis revealed that right superior parietal cortex was engaged to a greater extent for ASL than for English. We propose that right parietal cortex is involved in the visual-motoric transformation required for ASL. The production of both English prepositions and ASL nouns engaged Brocas area to a greater extent than ASL classifier constructions. We suggest that Brocas area is not engaged because these constructions do not involve retrieval of the name of an object or the name of a spatial relation. Finally, under the same task conditions, only left parietal activation was observed for monolingual English speakers producing spatial prepositions (H. Damasio et al., 2001, NeuroImage, 13). We conclude that the right hemisphere activation observed for ASL-English bilinguals was due to their life-long experience with spatial language in ASL.


Neuropsychologia | 2003

Neural systems underlying lexical retrieval for sign language.

Karen Emmorey; Thomas J. Grabowski; Stephen McCullough; Hanna Damasio; Laura L. Boles Ponto; Richard D. Hichwa; Ursula Bellugi

Positron emission tomography was used to investigate whether signed languages exhibit the same neural organization for lexical retrieval within classical and non-classical language areas as has been described for spoken English. Ten deaf native American sign language (ASL) signers were shown pictures of unique entities (famous persons) and non-unique entities (animals) and were asked to name each stimulus with an overt signed response. Proper name signed responses to famous people were fingerspelled, and common noun responses to animals were both fingerspelled and signed with native ASL signs. In general, retrieving ASL signs activated neural sites similar to those activated by hearing subjects retrieving English words. Naming famous persons activated the left temporal pole (TP), whereas naming animals (whether fingerspelled or signed) activated left inferotemporal (IT) cortex. The retrieval of fingerspelled and native signs generally engaged the same cortical regions, but fingerspelled signs in addition activated a premotor region, perhaps due to the increased motor planning and sequencing demanded by fingerspelling. Native signs activated portions of the left supramarginal gyrus (SMG), an area previously implicated in the retrieval of phonological features of ASL signs. Overall, the findings indicate that similar neuroanatomical areas are involved in lexical retrieval for both signs and words.


Journal of the Neurological Sciences | 1985

Blink reflex in patients with hemispheric cerebrovascular accident (CVA): Blink reflex in CVA

Jun Kimura; J. Terry Wilkinson; Hanna Damasio; Harold R. Adams; Essatollah Shivapour; Thoru Yamada

A blink reflex consists of an early unilateral component, R1, and a late bilateral component, R2. During an acute phase of hemispheric cerebrovascular accident, R1 and R2 were abnormal in 30 and 50 of 66 patients, respectively. Paired stimuli usually corrected R1 but not R2, which was profoundly suppressed. The discrepancy between polysynaptic R2 and oligosynaptic R1 indicates a greater disfacilitation at the level of interneurons than at the motoneuron, which serves as the final common path. Abnormality of R2 occurred bilaterally with stimulation on the affected side of face and contralaterally after stimulation on the normal side in 31 patients. This finding suggests a diffuse loss of internuncial excitability, contralateral to the hemispheric lesion. Changes of R2 implicated the brainstem pathways forming the afferent and efferent arc of the reflex in 7 and 8 patients, respectively. The remaining 4 comatose patients had no R2 irrespective of stimulus sites. Clinical localization of the hemispheric lesion showed no consistent correlation with the type of blink reflex abnormalities. The CT scans revealed widely scattered changes in 29 patients with abnormal blink reflex but with a tendency to overlap in the inferior Rolandic area. This contrasted with conspicuous sparing of the inferior post-central region in 10 patients with normal blink reflex. These findings suggest the presence of crossed facilitation to this reflex from wide areas of the cortex but most prominently from the sensory representation of the face.


Neuropsychologia | 2003

Neural correlates of naming animals from their characteristic sounds

Daniel Tranel; Hanna Damasio; Gerald R Eichhorn; Thomas J. Grabowski; Laura L. Boles Ponto; Richard D. Hichwa

The neural correlates of naming stimuli presented through the auditory modality have scarcely been studied. Using a PET experiment in 10 normal subjects, we began to address this issue by testing the hypothesis that naming animals from their characteristic sounds will engage bilateral primary auditory and auditory association cortices, bilateral early visual association cortices, left inferotemporal (IT) cortices, and left frontal operculum. Subjects listened to characteristic animal sounds (e.g. a rooster crowing), and named the animals making the sounds. When contrasted with a baseline task that involved saying up/down to the direction of pitch change in tone sequences, the naming task produced activation in mesial occipital cortices, the left ventral IT region, and the left frontal operculum. We interpret the activation in visual association cortices to reflect the process of retrieving conceptual knowledge (e.g. physical structure) pertinent to the animals being named, as in visual images. The left IT activation is interpreted to reflect activation of a mediation system for word retrieval, that operates to link conceptual knowledge retrieval to word production, and whose triggering is independent of the sensory modality in which a stimulus is presented.

Collaboration


Dive into the Hanna Damasio's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Antonio R. Damasio

Brain and Creativity Institute

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John S. Allen

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ralph Adolphs

California Institute of Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Antoine Bechara

Roy J. and Lucille A. Carver College of Medicine

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Albert M. Galaburda

Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge