Hans C. Dringenberg
Queen's University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Hans C. Dringenberg.
European Journal of Neuroscience | 2002
D.P Richardson; M.L. Byrnes; James F. Brien; James N. Reynolds; Hans C. Dringenberg
In the hippocampus, the CA1 region is selectively vulnerable to the effects of chronic prenatal ethanol exposure. In the guinea‐pig, the number of CA1 pyramidal cells is decreased after chronic prenatal ethanol exposure. We tested the hypotheses that chronic prenatal ethanol exposure (through maternal ethanol ingestion) results in impairments in spatial learning and short‐ and long‐term plasticity in the CA1 region of the postnatal guinea‐pig hippocampus. Timed, pregnant guinea‐pigs were treated with ethanol (4 g/kg maternal body weight/day), isocaloric sucrose/pair‐feeding, or water throughout gestation. Offspring were studied between postnatal days 40 and 80. In the Morris water maze, animals exposed to ethanol prenatally showed slower acquisition of an escape response to a hidden platform over 5 days of training. The amplitude of the field excitatory postsynaptic potential in the CA1 region in response to contralateral CA3 stimulation was decreased in offspring exposed to ethanol prenatally. Two forms of short‐term plasticity (paired‐pulse and frequency facilitation) were unaffected by chronic prenatal ethanol exposure. Long‐term potentiation (LTP) in response to high‐frequency CA3 stimulation was induced reliably and maintained over 60 min in isocaloric‐sucrose and water control animals. However, LTP failed to be induced in the CA1 area of the hippocampus in prenatal ethanol‐exposed offspring. These data show that chronic prenatal ethanol exposure, through maternal ethanol administration, impairs spatial performance and LTP in CA1 neurons. Hippocampal dysfunction could contribute importantly to the cognitive and behavioural deficits resulting from chronic prenatal ethanol exposure.
Behavioural Brain Research | 2003
Tracie A. Paine; Hans C. Dringenberg; Mary C. Olmstead
Drug addiction can be considered an impulse control disorder in that addicts exhibit increased impulsivity on both behavioural and self-report measures. We investigated whether chronic cocaine affects delay of gratification and/or behavioural disinhibition in rats using the delayed reinforcement and Go/No-go paradigms. Animals were treated with saline or cocaine (15 mg/kg) three times per day for 14 days; all behavioural tests occurred prior to daily injections. To assess the effectiveness of the cocaine treatment, sucrose intake, behavioural sensitization and serotonin (5-HT)-dependent (dorsal raphe-stimulated) cortical activation were also measured. Chronic cocaine caused a transient (days 7-8) increase in impulsivity in the delayed reinforcement paradigm, but did not influence behaviour in the Go/No-go paradigm. As expected, chronic cocaine increased behavioural sensitization scores, although it did not affect sucrose consumption. Although, cocaine treatment did not affect dorsal raphe-stimulated electrocorticographic activation, the serotonergic receptor antagonist methiothepin (0.1 mg/kg) was more effective in blocking cortical activation in cocaine- than in saline-treated animals. The electrocorticographic changes may be the result of a pre-synaptic 5-HT deficit and the compensatory supersensitivity of post-synaptic 5-HT receptors. Given the differential time courses of the behavioural and electrocorticographic data, however, this change probably does not mediate the effects of chronic cocaine in the delayed reinforcement paradigm.
Neuroscience | 2003
Hans C. Dringenberg; Mary C. Olmstead
Efferents from the pedunculopontine tegmentum (PPTg) exert widespread control over neocortical electrocorticographic (ECoG) activity and aid in maintaining high-frequency ECoG activation during waking and rapid eye movement sleep. The mechanisms and subcortical routes that allow the PPTg to influence cortical activity remain controversial. We examined the relative contributions of the thalamus and basal forebrain in ECoG activation elicited by PPTg stimulation in urethane-anesthetized rats. Stimulation (100 Hz, 2 s) of the PPTg suppressed large-amplitude, low-frequency oscillations, replacing them with high-frequency beta-gamma activity. Systemic administration of the anti-muscarinic drug scopolamine (1 mg/kg, i.p.) abolished activation elicited by PPTg stimulation, suggestive of an essential role of acetylcholine in this effect. Local infusions of lidocaine (1 microl, 1%) into the region of the cholinergic basal forebrain complex produced a strong reduction in activation elicited by PPTg stimulation. Lidocaine infusions into the reticular thalamic nucleus had no effect, but infusions into central thalamus produced a small attenuation of PPTg-evoked cortical activation. Combined basal forebrain-central thalamic infusions (1 microl/site) produced roughly additive effects, leading to a greater loss of activation than single-site infusions. These results indicate that, under the present experimental conditions, high-frequency cortical ECoG activation elicited by the PPTg involves relays in both the basal forebrain and central thalamus, with a predominant role of the basal forebrain. After concurrent central thalamic-basal forebrain inactivation, the forebrain can maintain only limited, short-lasting activation in response to PPTg stimulation. The additivity of infusion effects suggests that, rather than participating in one serial system, basal forebrain and central thalamus constitute parallel activating pathways. These findings aid in resolving previous controversies regarding the role of thalamus and basal forebrain in activation by emphasizing the importance of multiple, large-scale networks between brainstem and cortex in regulating the activation state of the mammalian neocortex.
Behavioural Brain Research | 2004
Umar Iqbal; Hans C. Dringenberg; James F. Brien; James N. Reynolds
Chronic prenatal ethanol exposure (CPEE) can injure the developing brain, and may lead to the fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS). Previous studies have demonstrated that CPEE upregulates gamma-aminobutyric acid type A (GABA(A)) receptor expression in the cerebral cortex, and decreases functional synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus, in the adult guinea pig. This study tested the hypothesis that CPEE increases GABA(A) receptor expression in the hippocampus of guinea pig offspring that exhibit cognitive deficits in a hippocampal-dependent spatial learning task. Timed, pregnant guinea pigs were treated with ethanol (4 g/kg maternal body weight per day), isocaloric-sucrose/pair-feeding, or water throughout gestation. GABA(A) receptor subunit protein expression in the hippocampus was measured at two development ages: near-term fetus and young adult. In young adult guinea pig offspring, CPEE increased spontaneous locomotor activity in the open-field and impaired task acquisition in the Morris water maze. CPEE did not change GABA(A) receptor subunit protein expression in the near-term fetal hippocampus, but increased expression of the beta2/3-subunit of the GABA(A) receptor in the hippocampus of young adult offspring. CPEE did not change either [(3)H]flunitrazepam binding or GABA potentiation of [(3)H]flunitrazepam binding, but decreased the efficacy of allopregnanolone potentiation of [(3)H]flunitrazepam binding, to hippocampal GABA(A) receptors in adult offspring. Correlational analysis revealed a relationship between increased spontaneous locomotor activity and growth restriction in the hippocampus induced by CPEE. Similarly, an inverse relationship was found between performance in the water maze and the efficacy of allopregnanolone potentiation of [(3)H]flunitrazepam binding in the hippocampus. These data suggest that alterations in hippocampal GABA(A) receptor expression and pharmacological properties contribute to hippocampal-related behavioral and cognitive deficits associated with CPEE.
Naunyn-schmiedebergs Archives of Pharmacology | 1998
Hans C. Dringenberg; M. Angelica de Souza-Silva; Rainer K.W. Schwarting; Joseph P. Huston
The dopaminergic system plays a central role in the processing of reward or reinforcement since drugs that have reinforcing properties all share the ability to elevate dopamine (DA) levels in the nucleus accumbens or neostriatum. Histamine H1 receptor antagonists are known to have reinforcing effects in humans and laboratory rats. Here, we examined the effect of systemic (i.p.) treatment with two H1 antagonists, chlorpheniramine and pyrilamine, on the extracellular levels of DA and its metabolites dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) and homovanillic acid (HVA) in the neostriatum and nucleus accumbens of urethane-anesthetized rats. Dopamine and metabolites were measured using in vivo microdialysis and HPLC with electrochemical detection. Saline injections did not produce significant effects on DA, DOPAC, or HVA levels in the neostriatum or nucleus accumbens. In the neostriatum, chlorpheniramine administration (5 and 20 mg/kg) produced a sustained increase in DA to approximately 140 and 180% of pre-injection baseline levels, respectively. In the nucleus accumbens, chlorpheniramine (20 mg/kg) produced a transient increase in DA levels to about 300% of baseline. In both the neostriatum and nucleus accumbens, DOPAC and HVA decreased after chlorpheniramine treatment. Pyrilamine administration (10 and 20 mg/kg) produced a sustained increase in neostriatal DA levels to 140 and 165%, respectively, and accumbens DA increased transiently to 230% after a dose of 20 mg/kg. Levels of neostriatal and accumbens DOPAC and HVA decreased after pyrilamine treatment. These results show that H1 antagonists can potently enhance DA levels in the neostriatum and nucleus accumbens of urethane-anesthetized rats. The neurochemical effects on DA and its metabolites seen here (increased DA, decreased DOPAC and HVA) are similar to those commonly observed with drugs of abuse (e.g. psychostimulants). The interaction of H1 antagonists with dopaminergic transmission may explain the reinforcing effects and abuse potential associated with these compounds.
Neuroreport | 2000
Sheri Smith; Hans C. Dringenberg; Brian M. Bennett; Gregory R. J. Thatcher; James N. Reynolds
The objective of this study was to test the hypothesis that activation of soluble guanylyl cyclase and increased cGMP formation in the brain would improve task acquisition in cognitively impaired animals. We evaluated the effects of a novel nitrate ester, GT 715 (2,3-dinitrooxy-(2,3-bis-nitrooxy-propyldisulfanyl)-propane), in scopolamine-induced impairment of task acquisition in the Morris water maze. GT 715 improved task acquisition in scopolamine-pretreated animals in a time-and dose-dependent manner, whereas the prototypical nitrate ester, glyceryl trinitrate (GTN), was ineffective. GT 715 also was more effective and more potent than GTN for activation of hippocampal guanylyl cyclase. The results of this study therefore suggest that stimulation of cerebral soluble guanylyl cyclase activity may be an effective strategy to improve learning and memory performance in individuals in whom cognitive abilities are impaired by injury, disease, or ageing.
European Journal of Neuroscience | 2007
William Speechley; Jennifer L. Hogsden; Hans C. Dringenberg
Long‐term potentiation (LTP) and long‐term depression are thought to mediate activity‐dependent brain plasticity but their role in the development of the thalamocortical auditory system in vivo has not been investigated. In adult urethane‐anaesthetized rats, theta‐burst stimulation of the medial geniculate nucleus produced robust LTP (40% amplitude enhancement) of field post‐synaptic evoked potentials recorded in the superficial layers of the primary auditory cortex. Low‐frequency (1‐Hz) stimulation resulted in transient depression (∼40%) of field post‐synaptic evoked potential amplitude. Both LTP and synaptic depression were found to be dependent on cortical N‐methyl‐d‐aspartate receptors. Thalamocortical plasticity was also assessed after continuous white noise exposure, thought to arrest auditory cortex maturation when applied during the critical period of post‐natal primary auditory cortex development. Rats housed in continuous white noise for the first 50 days of post‐natal life exhibited greater LTP (∼80%) than controls reared in unaltered acoustic environments. The protocol used to elicit depression also resulted in substantial LTP (∼50%) in white noise‐reared animals. Adults housed in white noise for the same length of time exhibited normal LTP but displayed greater and persistent levels of synaptic depression (∼70%). Thus, the absence of patterned auditory stimulation during early post‐natal life appears to retard sensory‐dependent thalamocortical synaptic strengthening, as indicated by the preferential readiness for synaptic potentiation over depression. The fact that the same auditory manipulation in adults results in synapses favouring depression demonstrates the critical role of developmental stage in determining the direction of synaptic modification in the thalamocortical auditory system.
European Journal of Neuroscience | 2004
Hans C. Dringenberg; Min-Ching Kuo; Summer Tomaszek
Synaptic potentiation allows neurons to enhance excitability and store information for extended time periods. We examined the role of the amygdaloid complex, known to facilitate long‐term memory encoding, to influence synaptic strength at thalamo‐cortical synapses. In urethane‐anaesthetized rats, theta‐burst stimulation of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus induced early phase (1–2 h) long‐term potentiation (LTP) of the field postsynaptic potential (fPSP) recorded in the ipsilateral primary visual cortex. Electrical stimulation (100 Hz) of the amygdala 5 min after thalamic stimulation converted early phase LTP to stable late‐phase (> 4 h) LTP. This effect was not correlated with the degree of electrocorticographic activation of V1 induced by amygdala stimulation. Amygdala stimulation without thalamic theta‐burst stimulation did not change thalamo‐cortical fPSPs. The centrally acting cholinergic‐muscarinic receptor antagonist scopolamine (1 mg/kg, i.p.), but not peripherally acting methyl‐scopolamine, completely blocked the amygdala‐induced conversion of early to late‐phase thalamo‐cortical LTP. Further, ventricular application of the transcription inhibitor anisomycin (250 µg) reduced amygdala‐induced late‐phase LTP induction. These results demonstrate that the amygdaloid complex transforms time‐limited synaptic enhancement of thalamo‐cortical transmission into long lasting increases in synaptic strength. These processes are mediated, at least in part, by cholinergic and transcription‐dependent mechanisms. These amygdaloid‐induced effects provide a potential mechanism underlying long‐term enhancement of sensory transmission and information encoding in thalamo‐cortical networks.
Journal of Neurochemistry | 2002
Hans C. Dringenberg; M. Angelica de Souza-Silva; Jens Roßmüller; Joseph P. Huston; Rainer K.W. Schwarting
Abstract: Lesions of the neuronal histaminergic system or pharmacological blockade of histamine receptors, e.g., with histamine H1 receptor antagonists, can enhance the performance of rats in several tests of learning and memory. The underlying neuronal systems that mediate these behavioral effects are not known. Here, we examined the effects of treatment with histamine H1 antagonists on extracellular levels of acetylcholine (ACh) in adult rats anesthetized with urethane (1.25 g/kg). ACh was quantified using in vivo microdialysis and HPLC with electrochemical detection. Basal levels of ACh in the frontal cortex and hippocampus were in the range of 0.54 ± 0.13 and 0.96 ± 0.17 pmol/20 min, respectively. Injection (intraperitoneally) of saline did not produce significant increases in ACh levels, even though there was a slight and gradual increase in cortical ACh levels throughout the course of the experiments (up to 4 h after an injection). Administration of the H1 receptor antagonist chlorpheniramine (intraperitoneally) produced a dose‐dependent increase of cortical ACh levels to a maximum of 260, 280, and 570% of baseline values after doses of 5, 10, and 20 mg/kg, respectively. In the hippocampus, ACh content increased to a maximum of ∼600% of baseline levels after chlorpheniramine administration (20 mg/kg, i.p.). Administration of the H1 antagonist pyrilamine (intraperitoneally) increased cortical ACh content to a maximum of 300 and 500%, whereas hippocampal ACh levels increased to 215 and 280% after doses of 10 and 20 mg/kg, respectively. In an additional experiment using nonanesthetized, freely moving rats, cortical ACh content showed a moderate increase (to 190%) after saline injections (intraperitoneally) and a much higher increase (to 370%) after chlorpheniramine treatment (20 mg/kg, i.p.). These data suggest that cortical and hippocampal levels of ACh can be effectively modulated by systemic treatment with histamine H1 antagonists. The increases in ACh levels produced by H1 antagonists may suggest that some histaminergic receptors exert an inhibitory influence over central ACh levels. The enhanced availability of ACh in the forebrain may contribute to the behavioral effects observed with H1 antagonist treatment.
Behavioural Brain Research | 2001
Hans C. Dringenberg; Devon P Richardson; James F. Brien; James N. Reynolds
This paper provides the first report of spatial learning in guinea pigs using the Morris water maze (MWM). Male and female guinea pigs were trained for 5 consecutive days (8 trials/day; acquisition phase) in either the visible (cued) or the hidden (non-cued) platform version of the MWM. In both tests, guinea pigs learned to navigate to the escape platform, as indicated by a decrease in escape latency over the 5 training days. There were no sex differences in either test version. A comparison of guinea pigs and male Wistar rats showed that performance during acquisition training was not different for the two species in the visible platform test, but rats performed better during the early training days in the hidden platform test. A retention test (probe trial) was given 5 days after the last acquisition training day. Again, there was no sex difference, and no difference between guinea pigs and rats. Finally, acquisition of a new escape response to a shifted platform location was equivalent for rats and guinea pigs of both sexes. These results demonstrate that guinea pigs show robust cued and non-cued learning in the MWM. Both acquisition and retention performance in guinea pigs is similar to that in rats, even though rats appear to have a slight advantage in the acquisition of non-cued, spatial information in this test. We conclude that the MWM provides a valuable paradigm to assess behavior and learning/memory in the guinea pig.