Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Hans-Christoph Nuerk is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Hans-Christoph Nuerk.


Cognition | 2001

Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins.

Hans-Christoph Nuerk; Ulrich Weger; Klaus Willmes

Most models of number recognition agree that among other number representations there is a central semantic magnitude representation which may be conceptualized as a logarithmically compressed mental number line. Whether or not this number line is decomposed into different representations for tens and units is, however, controversial. We investigated this issue in German participants in a magnitude comparison (selection) task in which the larger of two visually presented Arabic two-digit numbers had to be selected. Most importantly, we varied unit-decade-compatibility: a number pair was defined as compatible if the decade magnitude comparison and the unit magnitude comparison of the two numbers would lead to the same response (e.g. 52 and 67) and as incompatible if this was not the case (e.g. 47 and 62). While controlling for overall numerical distance, size and other variables, we consistently found compatibility effects. A control experiment showed that this compatibility effect was not due to perceptual presentation characteristics. We conclude that the idea of one single number line representation that does not additionally assume different magnitude representations for tens and units is not sufficient to account for the data. Finally, we discuss why decade effects were not found in other experimental settings.


Experimental Psychology | 2005

The Universal SNARC Effect: The Association between Number Magnitude and Space is Amodal.

Hans-Christoph Nuerk; Guilherme Wood; Klaus Willmes

It is thought that number magnitude is represented in an abstract and amodal way on a left-to-right oriented mental number line. Major evidence for this idea has been provided by the SNARC effect (Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993): responses to relatively larger numbers are faster for the right hand, those to smaller numbers for the left hand, even when number magnitude is irrelevant. The SNARC effect has been used to index automatic access to a central semantic and amodal magnitude representation. However, this assumption of modality independence has never been tested and it remains uncertain if the SNARC effect exists in other modalities in a similar way as in the visual modality. We have examined this question by systematically varying modality/notation (auditory number word, visual Arabic numeral, visual number word, visual dice pattern) in a within-participant design. The SNARC effect was found consistently for all modality/notation conditions, including auditory presentation. The size of the SNARC effect in the auditory condition did not differ from the SNARC effect in any visual condition. We conclude that the SNARC effect is indeed a general index of a central semantic and amodal number magnitude representation.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2009

Children's early mental number line: Logarithmic or decomposed linear?

Korbinian Moeller; Silvia Pixner; Liane Kaufmann; Hans-Christoph Nuerk

Recently, the nature of childrens mental number line has received much investigation. In the number line task, children are required to mark a presented number on a physical number line with fixed endpoints. Typically, it was observed that the estimations of younger/inexperienced children were accounted for best by a logarithmic function, whereas those of older/more experienced children were reflected best by a linear function. This led to the conclusion that childrens mental number line transforms from logarithmic to linear with age and experience. In this study, we outline an alternative interpretation of childrens performance in a number line task. We suggest that two separate linear representations for one- and two-digit numbers may exist in young children and that initially the integration of these two representations into the place value structure of the Arabic number system is not fully mastered. When testing this assumption in a sample of more than 120 first graders, we observed that the two-linear model consistently provided better fit indexes. We conclude that instead of assuming a transition from logarithmic to linear coding, performance differences could also be accounted for by an improvement in integrating tens and units into the Arabic place value system.


Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2011

Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation

Ursula Fischer; Korbinian Moeller; Martina Bientzle; Ulrike Cress; Hans-Christoph Nuerk

An adequately developed spatial representation of number magnitude is associated with children’s general arithmetic achievement. Therefore, a new spatial-numerical training program for kindergarten children was developed in which presentation and response were associated with a congruent spatial numerical representation. In particular, children responded by a full-body spatial movement on a digital dance mat in a magnitude comparison task. This spatial-numerical training was more effective than a non-spatial control training in enhancing children’s performance on a number line estimation task and a subtest of a standardized mathematical achievement battery (TEDI-MATH). A mediation analysis suggested that these improvements were driven by an improvement of children’s mental number line representation and not only by unspecific factors such as attention or motivation. These results suggest a benefit of spatial numerical associations. Rather than being a merely associated covariate, they work as an independently manipulated variable which is functional for numerical development.


Cortex | 2006

Crossed Hands and the Snarc Effect: Afailure to Replicate Dehaene, Bossini and Giraux (1993)

Guilherme Wood; Hans-Christoph Nuerk; Klaus Willmes

Dehaene et al. (1993, Experiment 6) presented evidence that the mental number line is left-to-right oriented with respect to representational associations and not with respect to left and right hands. Here we tried to replicate the study of Dehaene et al. (1993) in a larger sample (n = 32) using four different stimulus notations (Arabic numbers, number words, auditory number words, and dice patterns). As in the study by Dehaene et al. (1993), the spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect was examined with an incongruent hand assignment to left/right response keys (crossed hands). In contrast to Dehaene et al. (1993), we did not observe a SNARC effect in any condition. Power analyses revealed that n = 32 should have been large enough to detect SNARC effects of usual size. Furthermore, time-course analyses revealed no SNARC slope in faster and slower responses, so that the null effect could not be due to relatively slow responses with crossed hands. Joint analyses with previous data (Nuerk et al., 2005b) revealed significantly steeper SNARC slopes with congruent hand assignment, and no interaction between hand assignment and notation. Altogether, these findings suggest that the results of Dehaene et al. (1993) only hold under specific conditions. Differences between studies are discussed. We suggest that spatial context has an influence on the SNARC effect and that hand-based associations (and not only representational associations) are relevant for the SNARC effect.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2009

On the language specificity of basic number processing: Transcoding in a language with inversion and its relation to working memory capacity

Julia Zuber; Silvia Pixner; Korbinian Moeller; Hans-Christoph Nuerk

Transcoding Arabic numbers from and into verbal number words is one of the most basic number processing tasks commonly used to index the verbal representation of numbers. The inversion property, which is an important feature of some number word systems (e.g., German einundzwanzig [one and twenty]), might represent a major difficulty in transcoding and a challenge to current transcoding models. The mastery of inversion, and of transcoding in general, might be related to nonnumerical factors such as working memory resources given that different elements and their sequence need to be memorized and manipulated. In this study, transcoding skills and different working memory components in Austrian (German-speaking) 7-year-olds were assessed. We observed that inversion poses a major problem in transcoding for German-speaking children. In addition, different components of working memory skills were differentially correlated with particular transcoding error types. We discuss how current transcoding models could account for these results and how they might need to be adapted to accommodate inversion properties and their relation to different working memory components.


Trends in Neuroscience and Education | 2013

Walk the number line – An embodied training of numerical concepts

Tanja Link; Korbinian Moeller; Stefan Huber; Ursula Fischer; Hans-Christoph Nuerk

Abstract Basic numerical representations such as the spatial representation of number magnitude seem to develop during early childhood and predict later arithmetic abilities. Moreover, the concept of embodied cognition suggests that seemingly abstract representations may be based on bodily experiences. An embodied intervention program was developed addressing the spatial representation of number magnitude. First-graders were trained to indicate the position of a given number by walking to the estimated location of that number on a number line on the floor. This training was compared to an identical number line training without task-specific full-bodily experiences. Children showed more pronounced training effects after the embodied training than after the control training. These differential training effects even generalized partially to specific numerical competencies not trained directly. Thereby, these data corroborate beneficial effects of embodied processes for the training of seemingly abstract cognitive representations in general and for the amelioration of basic numerical representations in particular.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2013

Dyscalculia from a developmental and differential perspective.

Liane Kaufmann; M. Mazzocco; Ann Dowker; Michael von Aster; Silke M. Göbel; Roland H. Grabner; Avishai Henik; Nancy C. Jordan; Annette Karmiloff-Smith; Karin Kucian; Orly Rubinsten; Denes Szucs; Ruth S. Shalev; Hans-Christoph Nuerk

Developmental dyscalculia (DD) and its treatment are receiving increasing research attention. A PsychInfo search for peer-reviewed articles with dyscalculia as a title word reveals 31 papers published from 1991–2001, versus 74 papers published from 2002–2012. Still, these small counts reflect the paucity of research on DD compared to dyslexia, despite the prevalence of mathematical difficulties. In the UK, 22% of adults have mathematical difficulties sufficient to impose severe practical and occupational restrictions (Bynner and Parsons, 1997; National Center for Education Statistics, 2011). It is unlikely that all of these individuals with mathematical difficulties have DD, but criteria for defining and diagnosing dyscalculia remain ambiguous (Mazzocco and Myers, 2003). What is treated as DD in one study may be conceptualized as another form of mathematical impairment in another study. Furthermore, DD is frequently—but, we believe, mistakenly- considered a largely homogeneous disorder. Here we advocate a differential and developmental perspective on DD focused on identifying behavioral, cognitive, and neural sources of individual differences that contribute to our understanding of what DD is and what it is not.


Experimental Psychology | 2004

On the Perceptual Generality of the Unit-Decade Compatibility Effect

Hans-Christoph Nuerk; Ulrich W. Weger; Klaus Willmes

Number magnitude is assumed to be holistically represented along a single mental number line. Recently, we have observed a unit-decade-compatibility effect which is inconsistent with that assumption (Nuerk, Weger, & Willmes, 2001). In two-digit Arabic number comparison, we have demonstrated that compatible comparisons in which separate decade and unit comparisons lead to the same decision (32_47, 3 < 4 and 2 < 7) were faster than incompatible trials (37_52, 3 < 5, but 7 > 2). Because overall distance was matched, a holistic model could not account for the compatibility effect. However, one could argue that the compatibility effect was due to the specific vertical perceptual arrangement of the two-digit numbers in Nuerk et al.s (2001) experiment where the decade digits and unit digits were presented column-wise above each other. To examine this objection, we studied the perceptual generality of the compatibility effect with diagonal presentation. We replicated the compatibility effect with diagonal presentation. It is concluded that the compatibility effect is not due to encoding characteristics imposed by the perceptual setting of the original experiment. In particular, the assumption of an overall analog magnitude representation for two-digit numbers is not consistent with these data.


Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2013

Is the SNARC effect related to the level of mathematics? No systematic relationship observed despite more power, more repetitions, and more direct assessment of arithmetic skill

Krzysztof Cipora; Hans-Christoph Nuerk

The SNARC (spatial–numerical association of response codes) described that larger numbers are responded faster with the right hand and smaller numbers with the left hand. It is held in the literature that arithmetically skilled and nonskilled adults differ in the SNARC. However, the respective data are descriptive, and the decisive tests are nonsignificant. Possible reasons for this nonsignificance could be that in previous studies (a) very small samples were used, (b) there were too few repetitions producing too little power and, consequently, reliabilities that were too small to reach conventional significance levels for the descriptive skill differences in the SNARC, and (c) general mathematical ability was assessed by the field of study of students, while individual arithmetic skills were not examined. Therefore we used a much bigger sample, a lot more repetitions, and direct assessment of arithmetic skills to explore relations between the SNARC effect and arithmetic skills. Nevertheless, a difference in SNARC effect between arithmetically skilled and nonskilled participants was not obtained. Bayesian analysis showed positive evidence of a true null effect, not just a power problem. Hence we conclude that the idea that arithmetically skilled and nonskilled participants generally differ in the SNARC effect is not warranted by our data.

Collaboration


Dive into the Hans-Christoph Nuerk's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Elise Klein

RWTH Aachen University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Liane Kaufmann

Innsbruck Medical University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Stefan Huber

University of Regensburg

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge