Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Harvey S. Smallman is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Harvey S. Smallman.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 1995

A Contrast Ratio Constraint on Stereo Matching

Harvey S. Smallman; Suzanne P. McKee

Stereopsis employs differences in the location of features in the two eyes to reconstruct their relative depths. Computational models largely ignore the contrast of these features; they simply require them to be visible in each eye and to possess the same contrast polarity. With a competitive matching paradigm we show that only features with a certain ratio of contrasts in the two eyes match. Increasing the contrast in one eye requires proportionally more contrast in the other eye for matching. This contrast relation exactly parallels the relation found for dichoptic masking, which behaves unlike any other form of contrast masking. A strange consequence of this contrast ratio constraint is that a feature may be monocularly visible, yet unmatched because the contrast ratio has not been satisfied. In this case features are seen as faint unmatched ‘ghosts’ near the plane of fixation. In a competitive matching situation then, stereopsis acts as if it imposes a contrast threshold on matches; features failing to exceed the threshold remain unmatched. This is a simple and biologically plausible way for the system to eliminate false matches and to reduce matching ambiguity.


Vision Research | 1995

Fine-to-coarse scale disambiguation in stereopsis

Harvey S. Smallman

Spatial frequency selectivity has been incorporated into various theories of stereo matching, along with spatial scale interactions operating from coarse-to-fine spatial scales. We concentrate here on the role of fine scale information in the stereo matching process and show that fine scale information is capable of disambiguating matches made at coarser scales. An ambiguous coarse scale stimulus was created by presenting a low frequency (2 c/deg) sine wave in anti-phase to the two eyes, whose endpoints betrayed no information about which way the sine waves should be matched. It could be seen with crossed or uncrossed disparity equally validly and at chance from trial to trial. To this was added a fine scale (8 c/deg) filtered random dot stimulus specifying unambiguously a certain disparity. Observers judged the apparent depth of the two stimuli as the disparity of the fine scale stimulus was varied. The sine wave was usually perceived to have the same sign disparity as the fine scale stimulus. Depth matching with the two superimposed stimuli confirmed that the coarse scale stimulus was actually disambiguated, and seen with disparities equal to half its spatial period. The results suggest the operation of a cross-spatial scale matching disambiguation process, which can operate in a fine-to-coarse fashion.


Vision Research | 1998

The area of spatial integration for initial horizontal disparity vergence

Ariella V. Popple; Harvey S. Smallman; John M. Findlay

We investigated over what central area disparity in a random dot stereogram is integrated to stimulate an initial vergence response. Vergence was measured subjectively, with a forced choice dichoptic nonius vernier task following a brief (230 msec) stimulus presentation. Stimuli were random-dot stereograms showing a central circular disc of 12.5 min arc crossed retinal disparity in front of, and occluding, a same density fixation plane surround. The size of the disc was varied. All ten observers responded to the brief stimulus. Initial vergence increased with increasing disc diameter and, for nine out of ten subjects, reached a maximum with the disc ca 6 deg, suggesting this is the extent of the spatial integration region. Below 6 deg diameter, surround and target disparities were averaged together. Initial horizontal vergence responds automatically to a cyclopean target presented in the centre of gaze by pooling disparities within a limited but surprisingly large area.


Vision Research | 1997

Is stereopsis effective in breaking camouflage for moving targets

Suzanne P. McKee; Scott N. J. Watamaniuk; Julie M. Harris; Harvey S. Smallman; Douglas G. Taylor

It has been suggested that breaking camouflage is one of the major functions of stereopsis (Julesz, 1971). In this study, we found that stereopsis is less effective in breaking camouflage for moving targets than for static ones. Observers were asked to detect a single dot moving on a straight trajectory amidst identical noise dots in random motion. In the three-dimensional (3D) condition, the noise dots filled a cylindrical volume 5.7 cm in height and diameter; the trajectory signal dot moved on an oblique 3D trajectory through the center of the cylinder. In the two-dimensional (2D) control condition, observers viewed one half-image of the 3D cylinder binocularly. Surprisingly, trajectory detection in the 3D condition was only slightly better than in the 2D condition. Stereoscopic tuning for motion detection was also measured with a novel target configuration in which the random motion noise was presented in two depth planes that straddled the fixation plane where the trajectory target was presented. As the disparity between the noise planes and the fixation plane was increased, trajectory detection improved, reaching a peak between 6 and 12 arcmin, and then declining to the 2D level at larger disparities, where the noise became diplopic. Similar tuning measurements were made for detecting a static pattern, a string of five aligned dots presented in the fixation plane between two planes of static noise dots. Adding disparity to the noise planes produced a far greater improvement in static detection than in motion detection, for a comparable range of disparities (1.5-12 arcmin). We speculate that the temporal characteristics of the stereo system are not well suited for responding to moving targets, with the result that stereo does not greatly enhance motion detection in noise.


Vision Research | 2002

Visual function before and after the removal of bilateral congenital cataracts in adulthood

Ione Fine; Harvey S. Smallman; Peter G. Doyle; Donald I. A. MacLeod

Subject Peter Doyle (PD) had congenital bilateral cataracts removed at the age of 43. Pre-operatively PDs visual acuity was 20/80, with a resolution limit around 15 cpd, and he experienced monocular diplopia with high contrast stimuli. Post-operatively PDs visual acuity improved to approximately 20/40, with a resolution limit around 25 cpd. Using a variety of pre- and post-operative tests we have documented a wide range of neural adaptations to his limited and distorted visual input, and have found a limited amount of post-operative adaptation to his newly improved visual input. These results show that the human visual system is capable of significant adaptation to the particular optical input that is experienced.


Perception | 1995

The ‘Uniqueness Constraint’ and Binocular Masking

Suzanne P. McKee; Mary J. Bravo; Harvey S. Smallman; Gordon E. Legge

In stereo-matching algorithms, the ‘uniqueness constraint’ requires that a feature in one stereo half-image be matched to, at most, one similar feature in the other half-image. Experiments are reported in which binocular contrast thresholds and depth-discrimination judgments have been used to determine whether the human stereo system makes unique matches. A single high-spatial-frequency target in the left eye was paired stereoscopically with two identical targets, presented near retinal correspondence (±3.5 min of disparity), in the right eye. Contrast-increment thresholds were measured for each of the targets in the right eye, and it was found that the target in the left eye masked both. Indeed, the amount of binocular masking for each member of the double target nearly equaled the masking observed when only a single target was presented to the right eye. Depth judgments confirmed that the target in the left eye had been matched to both targets in the right eye. It is concluded that uniqueness is not an absolute constraint on human stereo matching.


Journal of The Optical Society of America A-optics Image Science and Vision | 1997

Fine-scale processing in human binocular stereopsis

Julie M. Harris; Suzanne P. McKee; Harvey S. Smallman

Many studies have demonstrated that the human visual system is sensitive to very small differences in relative binocular disparity. It is not known over what monocular regions information is spatially integrated to mediate performance in such tasks. In this study we present psychophysical observations that define the smallest spatial scale involved in disparity processing, and we indicate the nature of the computations performed by the units mediating that disparity discrimination. We show that human observers can identify the sign of disparity of a single target dot when it is embedded in a row of identical dots, with these noise dots presented either in the fixation plane or with a proportion binocularly uncorrelated. In conjunction with the psychophysical data, we explore how a class of simple correlator models of stereopsis must be constrained in order to account for human performance for the same fine-scale tasks. Such models can perform the task only when the correlation is carried out over a very small region of the image, for a very small range of disparities. Our results demonstrate that there is a fine-scale input to the stereo system, mediated by foveal mechanisms that spatially integrate visual signals over a region as small as 4-6 arcmin in diameter.


Displays | 1993

On the usefulness of basic colour coding in an information display

Harvey S. Smallman; Robert M. Boynton

Abstract Basic colours segregate well in displays, but no better than non-basic ones equally well separated in colour space. Basic colour coding was compared with an individuals preferred code made up of a personal choice of colours. These codes yielded equally good segragation when assessed in a visual search task. However, when tested on another persons codes, with which they had had no previous experience, there was a suggestion that subjects were quicker to learn the basic than the idiosyncratic code. When coding qualitative data in a crowded display the authors advocate a code made up of the users internally-generated set of basic colours. This code is easy to generate and obviates the need for complicated colour calibration procedures.


Perception | 1997

Spatial scale interactions in stereo sensitivity and the neural representation of binocular disparity.

Harvey S. Smallman; Donald I. A. MacLeod

How are binocular disparities encoded and represented in the human visual system? An ‘encoding cube’ diagram is introduced to visualise differences between competing models. To distinguish the models experimentally, the depth-increment-detection function (discriminating disparity d from d ± Δd) was measured as a function of standing disparity (d) with spatially filtered random-dot stereograms of different centre spatial frequencies. Stereothresholds degraded more quickly as standing disparity was increased with stimuli defined by high rather than low centre spatial frequency. This is consistent with a close correlation between the spatial scale of detection mechanisms and the disparities they process. It is shown that a simple model, where discrimination is limited by the noisy ratio of outputs of three disparity-selective mechanisms at each spatial scale, can account for the data. It is not necessary to invoke a population code for disparity to model the depth-increment-detection function. This type of encoding scheme implies insensitivity to large interocular phase differences. Might the system have developed a strategy to disambiguate or shift the matches made at fine scales with those made at the coarse scales at large standing disparities? In agreement with Rohaly and Wilson, no evidence was found that this is so. Such a scheme would predict that stereothresholds determined with targets composed of compounds of high and low frequency should be superior to those of either component alone. Although a small stereoacuity benefit was found at small disparities, the more striking result was that stereothresholds for compound-frequency targets were actually degraded at large standing disparities. The results argue against neural shifting of the matching range of fine scales by coarse-scale matches posited by certain stereo models.


Nature | 2001

Vision: Realignment of cones after cataract removal

Harvey S. Smallman; Donald I. A. MacLeod; Peter G. Doyle

Through unique observations of an adult case of bilateral congenital cataract removal, we have found evidence that retinal photoreceptors will swiftly realign towards the brightest regions in the pupils of the eye. Cones may be phototropic, actively orientating themselves towards light like sunflowers in a field.

Collaboration


Dive into the Harvey S. Smallman's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Suzanne P. McKee

Smith-Kettlewell Institute

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ione Fine

University of Washington

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Douglas G. Taylor

Smith-Kettlewell Institute

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge