Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where J. van der Steen is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by J. van der Steen.


Experimental Brain Research | 1985

Human ocular counterroll: assessment of static and dynamic properties from electromagnetic scleral coil recordings

H. Collewijn; J. van der Steen; L. Ferman; T. C. Jansen

SummaryStatic and dynamic components of ocular counterroll as well as cyclorotatory optokinetic nystagmus were measured with a scleral search coil technique. Static counterroll compensated for about 10% of head roll when the head was tilted to steady positions up to 20 deg from the upright position. The dynamic component of counterroll, which occurs only while the head is moving, is much larger. It consists of smooth compensatory cyclorotation opposite to the head rotation, interrupted frequently by saccades moving in the same direction as the head. During voluntary sinusoidal head roll, cyclorotation compensated from 40% to more than 70% of the head motion. In the range 0.16 to 1.33 Hz, gain increased with frequency and with the amount of visual information. The lowest values were found in darkness. The gain increased in the presence of a visual fixation point and a further rise was induced by a structured visual pattern. Resetting saccades were made more frequently in the dark than in the light. These saccades were somewhat slower than typical horizontal saccades. Cyclorotatory optokinetic nystagmus could be induced by a patterned disk rotating around the visual axis. It was highly variable even within a same subject and had in general a very low gain (mean value about 0.03 for stimulus velocities up to 30 deg/s). It is concluded that cyclorotational slip velocity on the retina is considerably reduced by counterroll during roll of the head, although the residual cyclorotation after the head has reached a steady position is very small.


Documenta Ophthalmologica | 1992

Foveation dynamics in congenital nystagmus I: Fixation

L. F. Dell'osso; J. van der Steen; Robert M. Steinman; H. Coleewijn

Congenital nystagmus (CN) has been described as a ‘fixation’ nystagmus implying an inability to fixate a target. However, each cycle of CN contains a target-foveation period during which the eye velocity is at, or near, zero. Prolongation of foveation time, reduction of retinal image velocity and cycle-to-cycle foveation repeatability all contribute to increased visual acuity. We developed several methods to accurately measure the dynamics of foveation in CN; their use is illustrated on an individual with typical idiopathic CN and no afferent defects. During eight 5-second intervals of fixation on a stationary target, the horizontal standard deviation (SD) of the mean foveation position (FPOS) was 12.82 minarc and the SD of foveation velocity was 118.36 minarc/sec. The SD of the means of total eye position and of the non-foveating peak of the CN were 43.17 and 25.32 minarc respectively. The mean foveation-time interval (eye velocity ⩽4°/sec) was 57.27 msec. The SD FPOS for the best 1-second interval (4 successive CN cycles), in a typical 5-second record, was 0.71 minarc. Histograms revealed peaks of eye position at 0 ± 10 minarc and of eye velocity at 0 ± 240 minarc/sec. The small vertical component of the CN (16 minarc peak-to-peak) had a SD of 6.56 minarc. A nystagmus foveation function related to visual acuity was derived that was more sensitive than CN intensity. The increased visual acuity resulting from the use of convergence or base-out prisms was due to increased foveation time. Although it might appear that CN is a defect of fixation, this individual with CN had strong fixation reflexes in the sense that he was able to accurately (within 1 minarc) achieve (interbeat) and maintain (intrabeat) target foveation for appreciable periods of time. Our data support the hypothesis that individuals with idiopathic CN do not have a primary disturbance of fixation.


Medical Engineering & Physics | 2009

Platform accelerations of three different whole-body vibration devices and the transmission of vertical vibrations to the lower limbs.

J.J.M. Pel; J. Bagheri; L.M. van Dam; H. J. G. van den Berg-Emons; H.L.D. Horemans; Henk J. Stam; J. van der Steen

Physical whole-body vibration (WBV) exercises become available at various levels of intensity. In a first series of measurements, we investigated 3-dimensional platform accelerations of three different WBV devices without and with three volunteers of different weight (62, 81 and 100 kg) in squat position (150 degrees knee flexion). The devices tested were two professional devices, the PowerPlate and the Galileo-Fitness, and one home-use device, the PowerMaxx. In a second series of measurements, the transmission of vertical platform accelerations of each device to the lower limbs was tested in eight healthy volunteers in squat position (100 degrees knee flexion). The first series showed that the platforms of two professional devices vibrated in an almost perfect vertical sine wave at frequencies between 25-50 and 5-40 Hz, respectively. The platform accelerations were slightly influenced by body weight. The PowerMaxx platform mainly vibrated in the horizontal plane at frequencies between 22 and 32 Hz, with minimal accelerations in the vertical direction. The weight of the volunteers reduced the platform accelerations in the horizontal plane but amplified those in the vertical direction about eight times. The vertical accelerations were highest in the Galileo (approximately 15 units of g) and the PowerPlate (approximately 8 units of g) and lowest in the PowerMaxx (approximately 2 units of g). The second series showed that the transmission of vertical accelerations at a common preset vibration frequency of 25 Hz were largest in the ankle and that transmission of acceleration reduced approximately 10 times at the knee and hip. We conclude that large variation in 3-dimensional accelerations exist in commercially available devices. The results suggest that these differences in mechanical behaviour induce variations in transmissibility of vertical vibrations to the (lower) body.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences | 1989

Ocular vergence under natural conditions. I. Continuous changes of target distance along the median plane.

Casper J. Erkelens; J. van der Steen; Robert M. Steinman; H. Collewijn

Horizontal binocular eye movements of four subjects were recorded with the scleral sensor coil - revolving magnetic field technique while they fixated a natural target, whose distance was varied in a normally illuminated room. The distance of the target relative to the head of the subject was changed in three ways: (a) the target was moved manually by the experimenter; (b) the target was moved manually by the subject; (c) the target remained stationary while the subject moved his upper torso towards and away from the target. The rate of change of target distance was varied systematically in four levels, ranging from ‘slow’ to ‘very fast’, corresponding to changes in target vergence from about 10° s-1 to about 100° s-1. The dynamics of ocular vergence with regard to delay and speed were, under all three conditions, considerably better than could be expected from the literature on ocular vergence induced by disparity and/or blur. When ‘very fast’ changes in the distance of the target were made, subjects achieved maximum vergence speeds of up to about 100° s-1. Delays of these fast vergence responses were generally smaller than 125 ms. Negative delays, i. e. ocular vergence leading the change in target distance, were observed. The eyes led the target (i. e. predicted target motion) by about 90 ms on average, when the subject used his hand to move the target. Vergence tracking was almost perfect when changes in distance were produced by moving the upper torso. In this condition, the eye led the target by about 5 ms. In the ‘slow’ and ‘medium’ conditions (stimulus speeds about 10-40° s-1) tracking was accurate to within 1-2°, irrespective of the way in which the target was moved. In the ‘fast’ and ‘very fast’ conditions (stimulus speeds about 40-100° s-1), the accuracy of vergence tracking was better for self-induced than for experimenter-induced target displacements, and accuracy was best during voluntary movements of the upper torso. In the last case, ocular vergence speed was within about 10% of the rate of change of the vergence angle formed by the eyes and the stationary target. The dynamics of convergent and divergent vergence responses varied considerably. These variations were idiosyncratic. They were consistent within, but not between, subjects. Ocular vergence associated with attempted fixation of an imagined target, changing distance in darkness, could only be made by two of the four subjects. The changes they could make were unreliable and poorly correlated with changes in the distance of the imagined target. Vergence changes did not occur when the distance to the target, imagined in darkness, was varied by keeping the target stationary and moving the torso back and forth. In conclusion, when ocular vergence was studied under relatively natural conditions in which there were many cues to the distance of the target, oculomotor vergence was both much faster and much more accurate than could have been anticipated from previous studies done under more restricted stimulating conditions.


Acta Oto-laryngologica | 1997

Gain and Delay of Human Vestibulo-ocular Reflexes to Oscillation and Steps of the Head by a Reactive Torque Helmet: I. Normal Subjects

S. Tabak; H. Collewijn; L. M.J.J. Boumans; J. van der Steen

Vestibulo-ocular reflexes (VOR) were evaluated with a reactive torque helmet that imposed high-frequency oscillation (2-20 Hz) or step displacements of the head in the horizontal plane. The present paper describes the experimental and analytical techniques and the results for normal subjects, which will serve as a baseline for the evaluation of vestibular pathology. For comparison, manually controlled head steps were also applied, as described in the literature. Eye and head movements were recorded with magnetic search coils. Non-vestibular effects were avoided by the use of high stimulus frequencies and the analysis of short time-windows (< 100 msec) after steps. Helmet-imposed steps caused a virtually uniform head acceleration (average magnitude 770 degrees/sec) in the first 90 msec. This resulted in a linear relation between eye and head velocities; the gain and delay of the VOR could be calculated independently from the slope and offset of this relation. Such estimates appear more reliable than those obtained with conventional techniques. Normal subjects had a VOR gain of about 0.9 and a delay of about 5 msec. The results of sinusoidal head oscillation were in agreement with the results for steps. The responses to manually generated head steps agreed in general with those to helmet-induced steps, but because of the non-uniform acceleration they allowed a less exact analysis of function.


Vision Research | 1994

Instability of ocular torsion during fixation: Cyclovergence is more stable than cycloversion

L.J. Van Rijn; J. van der Steen; H. Collewijn

We investigated spontaneous variation of binocular torsion. Variation was expressed as SD of torsional eye positions measured over periods up to 32 sec. Subjects viewed a single dot target for periods of 32 sec. In half of the trials a large random-dot background pattern was superimposed on the dot. The movements of both eyes were measured with scleral induction coils. Spontaneous torsional movements were largely conjugate: cyclovergence was much more stable than cycloversion. This difference was not due to roll head movements. Stability of cyclovergence was improved by the background pattern. Although overall stability (SD of position) of cycloversion was unaffected by a background, the background induced or enhanced a small-amplitude torsional nystagmus in 3 out of 4 subjects. We hypothesize that the difference in stability of cycloversion vs cyclovergence reflects the greater importance of torsional retinal correspondence, compared to absolute torsional position. In two subjects we found evidence for the existence of cyclophoria, manifested by systematic shifts in cyclovergence caused by the appearance and disappearance of the background.


Vision Research | 1995

Unequal amplitude saccades produced by aniseikonic patterns: effects of viewing distance

J. van der Steen; P. Bruno

This study describes differences in horizontal and vertical disconjugate saccades under far and close viewing conditions of two dichoptically presented aniseikonic random checkerboard patterns. At far viewing, disconjugacy of horizontal saccades requiring divergence was accomplished intrasaccadically after several minutes; for convergence the intrasaccadic disconjugacy was limited. Size differences partially persisted in open-loop trials. At close viewing intrasaccadic divergent changes in conjugacy were instantaneous, but motor storage during open-loop was absent. It is concluded that disconjugate saccades to targets at far distance lead to an adaptation process, whereas at close viewing distance horizontal disparity is a visual compensation process used directly to scale the relative amplitudes of both eyes, not leading to adaptation. The time-course of disconjugate vertical saccades was much slower, with mostly postsaccadic vergence. Nearby viewing enhanced the disconjugacy of vertical saccades.


Experimental Brain Research | 1984

Ocular stability in the horizontal, frontal and sagittal planes in the rabbit

J. van der Steen; H. Collewijn

SummaryEye and head movements in the horizontal, frontal and sagittal planes were recorded in the rabbit with a newly developed technique using dual scleral search coils in a rotating magnetic field. The compensatory eye movements elicited by passive sinusoidal oscillation detoriated for frequencies below 0.1 Hz in the horizontal, but not in the frontal and sagittal planes. In the light gain was relatively independent of frequency in all planes and amounted to 0.82–0.69, 0.92–0.83 and 0.65–0.59 in the horizontal, frontal and sagittal plane, respectively. In freely moving animals, similar input-output relations were found. The stability of the retinal image thus proved to be inversely proportional to the amount of head movements associated with behavioural activity. Maximal retinal image velocities varied between 2–4°/s for a rabbit sitting quietly and 30–40°/s during locomotor activity. Gaze displacements showed different characteristics in the various planes, possibly in relation with the structure of the retinal visual streak. Horizontal gaze changes were mainly effected by saccades. Gaze changes in the frontal plane were relatively rare and effected by non-saccadic, combined head and eye movements with temporary suppression of compensatory eye movements. Eye rotations in the sagittal plane, possibly functioning to adjust the direction of binocular vision vertically, were abundant and effected by large head movements in combination with a low gain of compensatory eye movements in this plane.


Documenta Ophthalmologica | 1992

Foveation dynamics in congenital nystagmus II: Smooth pursuit

L. F. Dell'osso; J. van der Steen; Robert M. Steinman; H. Collewijn

It has been shown that, during 5 seconds of fixation, an individual with congenital nystagmus (CN) can repeatedly (beat-to-beat) foveate (SD = 12.87 minarc) and maintain low retinal slip velocities (SD = 118.36 minarc/sec). Smooth pursuit data from several CN subjects showed that eye velocities during these foveation intervals approximated target velocity. Despite some claims that CN is caused by absent or “reversed” smooth pursuit, those with CN hardly ever experience oscillopsia or exhibit any accompanying symptoms of such deficits in pursuit; they are able to master sports requiring tracking of rapidly moving small objects (e.g. racquetball or handball). We developed and describe several new methods to accurately assess the function of smooth pursuit in an individual with typical idiopathic CN. We investigated the dynamics of CN foveation periods during smooth pursuit to test the hypothesis that eye velocities would match target velocities during these periods. Unity or near-unity instantaneous (beat-to-beat) pursuit gains of both experimenter-moved and subject-moved targets at peak velocities ranging from only a few deg/sec up to 210°/sec were measured. The dynamic neutral zone was found to shift oppositely to target direction by amounts proportional to the increase in target speed. Our methods proved that eye velocity is made to match target velocity during the foveation intervals and support the conclusion that smooth pursuit in individuals with CN is functioning normally in the presence of the CN oscillation. In addition, we hypothesize that the same fixation mechanism that prevents oscillopsia during fixation of stationary targets, also does so during pursuit.


European Journal of Neuroscience | 2004

Simple spike and complex spike activity of floccular Purkinje cells during the optokinetic reflex in mice lacking cerebellar long-term depression

H. H. L. M. Goossens; Freek E. Hoebeek; A.M. van Alphen; J. van der Steen; John S. Stahl; C. I. De Zeeuw; Maarten A. Frens

Cerebellar long‐term depression (LTD) at parallel fibre–Purkinje cell (P‐cell) synapses is thought to embody neuronal information storage for motor learning. Transgenic L7‐protein kinase C inhibitor (PKCI) mice in which cerebellar LTD is selectively blocked do indeed exhibit impaired adaptation in the vestibulo‐ocular reflex (VOR) while their default oculomotor performance is unaffected. Although supportive, these data do not definitively establish a causal link between memory storage required for motor learning and cerebellar LTD. As the L7‐PKCI transgene is probably activated from the early stages of P‐cell development, an alternative could be that P‐cells develop abnormal signals in L7‐PKCI mutants, disturbing mechanisms of motor learning that rely on proper P‐cell outputs. To test this alternative hypothesis, we studied simple spike (SS) and complex spike (CS) activity of vertical axis P‐cells in the flocculus of L7‐PKCI mice and their wild‐type littermates during sinusoidal optokinetic stimulation. Both SS and CS discharge dynamics appeared to be very similar in wild‐type and transgenic P‐cells at all stimulus frequencies (0.05–0.8 Hz). The CS activity of all vertical axis cells increased with contralateral stimulus rotation and lagged ipsiversive eye velocity by 165–180°. The SS modulation was roughly reciprocal to the CS modulation and lagged ipsiversive eye velocity by ∼ 15°. The baseline SS and CS discharge characteristics were indistinguishable between the two genotypes. We conclude that the impaired VOR learning in L7‐PKCI mutants does not reflect fundamental aberrations of the cerebellar circuitry. The data thus strengthen the evidence that cerebellar LTD is implicated in rapid VOR learning but not in the development of normal default response patterns.

Collaboration


Dive into the J. van der Steen's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

H. Collewijn

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J.J.M. Pel

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

H. S. Tan

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

F. H. Boot

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

L.J. Van Rijn

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maarten A. Frens

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

A. L. Mathoera

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

C. de Boer

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

C.M.P.M. Hertogh

VU University Medical Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Heleen M. Evenhuis

Erasmus University Rotterdam

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge