Jacques Pellat
Joseph Fourier University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Jacques Pellat.
Reading and Writing | 2003
Sylviane Valdois; Marie-Line Bosse; Bernard Ans; Serge Carbonnel; Michel Zorman; Danielle David; Jacques Pellat
The present study describes two Frenchteenagers with developmental reading andwriting impairments whose performance wascompared to that of chronological age andreading age matched non-dyslexic participants.Laurent conforms to the pattern of phonologicaldyslexia: he exhibits a poor performance inpseudo-word reading and spelling, producesphonologically inaccurate misspellings butreads most exception words accurately. Nicolas,in contrast, is poor in reading and spelling ofexception words but is quite good atpseudo-word spelling, suggesting that hesuffers from surface dyslexia and dysgraphia.The two participants were submitted to anextensive battery of metaphonological tasks andto two visual attentional tasks. Laurentdemonstrated poor phonemic awareness skills butgood visual processing abilities, while Nicolasshowed the reverse pattern with severedifficulties in the visual attentional tasksbut good phonemic awareness. The presentresults suggest that a visual attentionaldisorder might be found to be associated withthe pattern of developmental surface dyslexia.The present findings further show thatphonological and visual processing deficits candissociate in developmental dyslexia.
European Neurology | 1996
Kotaro Oizumi; P. Baumann; P. Siira; H. Vanharanta; V.V. Myllylä; Ming-Jang Chiu; Rong-Chi Chen; Chiu-yu Tseng; A. Rossi; Tetsuya Iidaka; Torn Nakajima; Kazuyuki Kawamoto; Hirohumi Fukuda; Yoshio Suzuki; Tadayuki Maehara; Hiroyasu Shiraishi; T. Ohishi; K. Kushida; M. Takahashi; K. Kawana; T. Inoue; K. Yagi; G. Tribl; K. Howorka; G. Heger; P. Anderer; H. Thoma; J. Zeitlhofer; Jens D. Rollnik; E. Sindern
A double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicentre study was conducted to evaluate the efficacy of pentoxifylline (Trental) in patients with multi-infarct dementia (MID) according to DSM-III-R criteria. Men and women aged 45 years or older, with a Hachinski Ischemia Scale score > or = 7 and a Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of 10-25 at entry, and computed tomographic evidence of vascular disease were enrolled. A total of 289 patients were randomised to receive either oral pentoxifylline 400 mg t.i.d. or placebo for 9 months, and efficacy was assessed every 3 months. The primary outcome variable was the difference in scores between the two treatment groups, as measured on the Gottfries, Bråne, Steen (GBS) scale. Secondary outcome variables included the scores achieved on the Sandoz Clinical Assessment Geriatric (SCAG) scale and MMSE, and a battery of psychological and other tests. The intention-to-treat analysis for patients completing the study (n = 239) showed a statistically significant difference in the total GBS score in favour of pentoxifylline (improvement of 3.5 points, p = 0.028). A significant difference in the total GBS score in favour of pentoxifylline was even almost achieved in the intention-to-treat analysis for all evaluable patients (n = 269, improvement of 2.1 points, p = 0.065). It is concluded that treatment with pentoxifylline is beneficial for patients with MID, the global results of the GBS and SCAG scales being reinforced by significant improvements in those subscales specific for intellectual and cognitive function.
Neuropsychologia | 1998
Olivier Moreaud; Annik Charnallet; Jacques Pellat
The role of semantic knowledge in object utilisation is a matter of debate. It is usually presumed that access to semantic knowledge is a necessary condition for manipulation, but a few reports challenged this view. The existence of a direct, pre-semantic route from vision to action has been proposed. We report the case of a patient with a disorder of object use in everyday life, in the context of probable Alzheimers disease. This patient was also impaired when manipulating single objects. He showed a striking dissociation between impairment in object use and preserved capacity to perform symbolic and meaningless gestures. To elucidate the nature of the disorder, and to clarify the relations between semantic knowledge and object use, we systematically assessed his capacity to recognise, name, access semantic knowledge, and use 15 common objects. We found no general semantic impairment for the objects that were not correctly manipulated, and, more importantly, no difference between the semantic knowledge of objects correctly manipulated and objects incorrectly manipulated. These data, although not incompatible with the hypothesis of a direct route for action, are better accommodated by the idea of a distributed semantic memory, where different types of knowledge are represented, as proposed by Allport (Allport, D. A. Current perspectives in dysphasia, pp. 32-60. Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, 1985).
Brain and Language | 2001
Olivier Moreaud; Danielle David; Annik Charnallet; Jacques Pellat
Patients with Alzheimers disease (AD) produce a high rate of semantic errors when naming to confrontation. This is considered to be one of the many consequences of their semantic memory deficit. However, it has been shown, in aphasic patients with focal lesions, that semantic errors could arise from impairment to any one of the levels in the naming process. To check this hypothesis in AD, we assessed in 15 patients the capacity to name and access semantic knowledge (by multiple-choice probe questions) about 14 objects presented successively in the visual, tactile, auditory, and verbal modalities. In the visual naming task, 33 errors were recorded: 26 (78.8%) were semantic and 7 (21.2%) were unrelated errors. Of the 26 semantic errors, 8 were related to a deficit of the semantic knowledge related to the item and 17 to a deficit in the retrieval of the phonological form of the word. One was associated with a deficit of access to semantic knowledge in the visual modality. The 7 unrelated errors were associated with a loss of semantic knowledge for 4 and deficit of access to the phonological form for 3. In conclusion, this study shows that semantic errors do not systematically reflect a deficit of semantic knowledge in Alzheimers disease. It also seems that unrelated errors are more frequently related to semantic deficits than semantic errors in this population.
Cognitive Neuropsychology | 1995
Sylviane Valdois; Serge Carbonnel; Danielle David; Stéphane Rousset; Jacques Pellat
Abstract A case study is presented of a patient, EA, who demonstrated all the defining features of deep dysphasia. His repetition disorder was associated with surface dyslexia and deep dysgraphia. EA also showed a severely restricted phonological STM. His performance in both picture confrontation naming and writing-to-dictation paralleled his performance in repetition, whereas reading aloud and oral lexical decision were not influenced by the imageability of the word input. Further testing indicated that EA did not have difficulty in either perceiving or semantically processing spoken words. An exhaustive investigation of EAs cognitive functioning was first conducted by reference to Patterson and Shewells model (1987). Such a triple-route model can account for EAs overall performance by postulating multiple functional lesions. We alternatively show that EAs language profile could be accounted for within a highly interactive model of language processing incorporating most basic principles of connection...
Neurocase | 2001
Marie-Josèphe Tainturier; Olivier Moreaud; Danielle David; E. Charles Leek; Jacques Pellat
Two main hypotheses have been proposed regarding the role of phonology in written word production. According to the phonological mediation hypothesis, the retrieval of the lexical phonological representation of a word is an obligatory prerequisite to the retrieval of its spelling. Therefore, deficits to the phonological lexicon should affect both spoken and written picture naming. In contrast, the orthographic autonomy hypothesis posits that the lexical orthographic representations of words can be accessed without any necessary phonological mediation. In support of this view, cases of preserved written naming despite impaired lexical phonology have been reported following brain damage. In this report, we replicate this basic pattern of performance in case YP, a 60-year-old woman with a pattern of frontotemporal dementia. As her disease progressed, YP’s ability to write down the names of pictures remained very good despite a severe decline in oral naming. Further testing indicated that this deficit was not primarily due to an articulatory or post-lexical phonological deficit. YP’s case provides strong additional support for the orthographic autonomy hypothesis. The significance of this case with respect to the characterization of dementia syndromes is discussed.
Psychopharmacology | 1985
Pierre Pollak; Jean-Marc Gaio; Marc Hommel; Jacques Pellat; Jean Perret
Tiapride, a selective D2 dopaminergic receptor blocking agent from the substituted benzamide class, was evaluated in a blind video-controlled trial in 10 psychiatric patients with tardive dyskinesia. There was a significant decrease in dyskinesia with a parallel increase in parkinsonism. This relationship between two opposite effects on movement suggests a common pathophysiological basis lying on a reciprocal hyper- and hypoactivity of the dopaminergic striatal system. Nevertheless, other mechanisms may be involved, for the evolution of individual parkinsonian and dyskinesia scores is not necessarily opposite: the tiapride-induced parkinsonism was generally acceptable and in two cases, the dyskinesia scores were reduced without an increase in parkinsonism. Therefore, more dyskinetic patients have to be evaluated in long-term studies with tiapride, before this drug could be recommended in tardive dyskinesia, when dyskinetic movements become intolerable.
Sleep | 1995
Bernadette Naegele; Valérie Thouvard; Jean-Louis Pépin; Patrick Levy; Catherine Bonnet; Jean Perret; Jacques Pellat; Claude Feuerstein
Sleep | 1998
Bernadette Naegele; Jean-Louis Pépin; Patrick Levy; Catherine Bonnet; Jacques Pellat; Claude Feuerstein
European Neurology | 1996
Olivier Moreaud; Annik Charnallet; Danielle David; Luc Cinotti; Jacques Pellat