Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where James J. Clark is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by James J. Clark.


Psychological Science | 1997

To See or not to See: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes

Ronald A. Rensink; J. Kevin O'Regan; James J. Clark

When looking at a scene, observers feel that they see its entire structure in great detail and can immediately notice any changes in it. However, when brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene, a striking failure of perception is induced: Identification of changes becomes extremely difficult, even when changes are large and made repeatedly. Identification is much faster when a verbal cue is provided, showing that poor visibility is not the cause of this difficulty. Identification is also faster for objects considered to be important in the scene. These results support the idea that observers never form a complete, detailed representation of their surroundings. In addition, the results indicate that attention is required to perceive change, and that in the absence of localized motion signals, attention is guided on the basis of high-level interest.


Nature | 2001

Interactive memory systems in the human brain

Russell A. Poldrack; James J. Clark; Paré-Blagoev Ej; Daphna Shohamy; J. Creso Moyano; Catherine E. Myers; Mark A. Gluck

Learning and memory in humans rely upon several memory systems, which appear to have dissociable brain substrates. A fundamental question concerns whether, and how, these memory systems interact. Here we show using functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) that these memory systems may compete with each other during classification learning in humans. The medial temporal lobe and basal ganglia were differently engaged across subjects during classification learning depending upon whether the task emphasized declarative or nondeclarative memory, even when the to-be-learned material and the level of performance did not differ. Consistent with competition between memory systems suggested by animal studies and neuroimaging, activity in these regions was negatively correlated across individuals. Further examination of classification learning using event-related FMRI showed rapid modulation of activity in these regions at the beginning of learning, suggesting that subjects relied upon the medial temporal lobe early in learning. However, this dependence rapidly declined with training, as predicted by previous computational models of associative learning.


Nature | 1999

Change-blindness as a result of 'mudsplashes'.

J. Kevin O'Regan; Ronald A. Rensink; James J. Clark

Change-blindness, occurs when large changes are missed under natural viewing conditions because they occur simultaneously with a brief visual disruption, perhaps caused by an eye movement,, a flicker, a blink, or a camera cut in a film sequence. We have found that this can occur even when the disruption does not cover or obscure the changes. When a few small, high-contrast shapes are briefly spattered over a picture, like mudsplashes on a car windscreen, large changes can be made simultaneously in the scene without being noticed. This phenomenon is potentially important in driving, surveillance or navigation, as dangerous events occurring in full view can go unnoticed if they coincide with even very small, apparently innocuous, disturbances. It is also important for understanding how the brain represents the world.


Archive | 1990

Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems

James J. Clark; Alan L. Yuille

1 Introduction: The Role of Data Fusion in Sensory Systems.- 2 Bayesian Sensory Information Processing.- 3 Information Processing Using Energy Function Minimization.- 4 Weakly vs. Strongly Coupled Data Fusion: A Classification of Fusional Methods.- 5 Data Fusion Applied to Feature Based Stereo Algorithms.- 6 Fusing Binocular and Monocular Depth Cues.- 7 Data Fusion in Shape from Shading Algorithms.- 8 Temporal Aspects of Data Fusion.- 9 Towards a Constraint Based Theory of Sensory Data Fusion.


Visual Cognition | 2000

Picture Changes During Blinks: Looking Without Seeing and Seeing Without Looking

J. Kevin O'Regan; Heiner Deubel; James J. Clark; Ronald A. Rensink

Observers inspected normal, high quality colour displays of everyday visual scenes while their eye movements were recorded. A large display change occurred each time an eye blink occurred. Display changes could either involve “Central Interest” or “Marginal Interest” locations, as determined from descriptions obtained from independent judges in a prior pilot experiment. Visual salience, as determined by luminance, colour, and position of the Central and Marginal Interest changes were equalized. The results obtained were very similar to those obtained in prior experiments showing failure to detect changes occurring simultaneously with saccades, flicker, or “mudsplashes” in the visual scene: Many changes were very hard to detect, and Marginal Interest changes were harder to detect than Central Interest changes. Analysis of eye movements showed, as expected, that the probability of detecting a change depended on the eyes distance from the change location. However a surprising finding was that both for Central and Marginal Interest changes, even when observers were directly fixating the change locations (within 1 degree), more than 40% of the time they still failed to see the changes. It seems that looking at something does not guarantee you “see” it.


Vision Research | 2002

Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts.

Ziad M. Hafed; James J. Clark

Microsaccades, or tiny eye movements that take place during periods of fixation, have long been thought to be random artifacts of the oculomotor system. Here we demonstrate a possible link between microsaccades and covert attention shifts. We designed two psychophysical tasks involving spatial cues that had identical sensory stimuli but differing patterns of attentional benefits and costs. We found that microsaccades, rather than being randomly distributed, had directions that were directly correlated with the directions of covert attention shifts in the two tasks. Our results suggest that microsaccades occur because of subliminal activation of the oculomotor system by covert attention.


Visual Cognition | 2000

On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions

Ronald A. Rensink; J. Kevin O'Regan; James J. Clark

When brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene, a striking failure of perception is induced: The changes become extremely difficult to notice, even when they are large, presented repeatedly, and the observer expects them to occur (Rensink, O’Regan, & Clark, 1997). To determine the mechanisms behind this induced “change blindness”, four experiments examine its dependence on initial preview and on the nature of the interruptions used. Results support the proposal that representations at the early stages of visual processing are inherently volatile, and that focused attention is needed to stabilize them sufficiently to support the perception of change.


IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing | 1985

A transformation method for the reconstruction of functions from nonuniformly spaced samples

James J. Clark; Matthew R. Palmer; Peter D. Lawrence

The reconstruction of functions from their samples at nonuniformly distributed locations is an important task for many applications. This paper presents a sampling theory which extends the uniform sampling theory of Whittaker et al. [11] to include nonuniform sample distributions. This extension is similar to the analysis of Papoulis [15], who considered reconstructions of functions that had been sampled at positions deviating slightly from a uniform sequence. Instead of treating the sample sequence as deviating from a uniform sequence, we show that a more general result can be obtained by treating the sample sequence as the result of applying a coordinate transformation to the uniform sequence. It is shown that the class of functions reconstructible in this manner generally include nonband-limited functions. The two-dimensional uniform sampling theory of Petersen and Middle ton [16] can be similarly extended as is shown in this paper. A practical algorithm for performing reconstructions of two-dimensional functions from nonuniformly spaced samples is described, as well as examples illustrating the performance of the algorithm.


IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence | 1989

Authenticating edges produced by zero-crossing algorithms

James J. Clark

It is shown that zero-crossing edge detection algorithms can produce edges that do not correspond to significant image intensity changes. Such edges are called phantom or spurious. A method for classifying zero crossings as corresponding to authentic or phantom edges is presented. The contrast of an authentic edge is shown to increase and the contrast of phantom edges to decrease with a decrease in the filter scale. Thus, a phantom edge is truly a phantom in that the closer one examines it, the weaker it becomes. The results of applying the classification schemes described to synthetic and authentic signals in one and two dimensions are given. The significance of the phantom edges is examined with respect to their frequency and strength relative to the authentic edges, and it is seen that authentic edges are denser and stronger, on the average, than phantom edges. >


international conference on computer vision | 1988

Modal Control Of An Attentive Vision System

James J. Clark; Nicola J. Ferrier

Cambridge, MA A vision system for use in a mobile robot system, or in n fixed multi-tasking industrial robot requires attentive control. Attentive control refers to the process by which the direction of gaze of the visual sensors are determined, nlong with the determination of what processing is required to be applied to the sensed images based on the goals of the robot and the tasks it is performing. This paper describes the implementation of a. mnt,ion control system which allows the attentive control of a binocular vision system. Attentive inputs to the system specify the type of visual feedback that the oculo-motor control system will use. The MDL language developed by Brockett [7] i8 used to communicate between the attentive planner and the motion controller.

Collaboration


Dive into the James J. Clark's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alan L. Yuille

Johns Hopkins University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ronald A. Rensink

University of British Columbia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. Kevin O'Regan

Paris Descartes University

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge